- The Project Gutenberg EBook of The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals, by
- Charles Darwin
- This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere at no cost and with
- almost no restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or
- re-use it under the terms of the Project Gutenberg License included
- with this eBook or online at www.gutenberg.org
- Title: The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals
- Author: Charles Darwin
- Release Date: March, 1998 [EBook #1227]
- Last Updated: October 21, 2019
- Language: English
- Character set encoding: UTF-8
- *** START OF THIS PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK EMOTION IN MAN AND ANIMALS ***
- Produced by Charles Keller and David Widger
- THE EXPRESSION OF THE EMOTIONS IN MAN AND ANIMALS
- By Charles Darwin
- _With Photographic And Other Illustrations_
- New York
- D. Appleton And Company
- 1899
- CONTENTS
- DETAILED CONTENTS.
- ON THE EXPRESSION OF THE EMOTIONS IN MAN AND ANIMALS.
- INTRODUCTION.
- CHAPTER I. — GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.
- CHAPTER II. — GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION—_continued_.
- CHAPTER III. — GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION—_concluded_.
- CHAPTER IV. — MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS.
- CHAPTER V. — SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS.
- CHAPTER VI. — SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING.
- CHAPTER VII. — LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR.
- CHAPTER VIII. — JOY, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION.
- CHAPTER IX. —
- REFLECTION—MEDITATION-ILL-TEMPER—SULKINESS—DETERMINATION.
- CHAPTER X. — HATRED AND ANGER.
- CHAPTER XI. — DISDAIN—CONTEMPT—DISGUST-GUILT—PRIDE, ETC.
- CHAPTER XII. — SURPRISE—ASTONISHMENT—FEAR—HORROR.
- CHAPTER XIII. — SELF-ATTENTION—SHAME—SHYNESS—MODESTY: BLUSHING.
- CHAPTER XIV. — CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY.
- FOOTNOTES
- ILLUSTRATIONS
- Muscles of the Human Face. Fig 1-2
- Muscles of the Human Face. Fig 3
- Small Dog Watching a Cat on A Table. Figure 4
- Dog in a Hostile Frame of Mind. Fig. 5
- Dog in a humble and Affectionate Frame of Mind. Fig. 6
- Dog in a Hostile Frame of Mind. Fig. 7
- Dog Carressing his Master. Fig. 8
- Cat, Savage, and Prepared to Fight. Fig. 9
- Cat in an Affectionate Frame of Mind. Fig. 10
- Sound Producing Quills from Tail of a Porcupine. Fig. 11
- Hen Driving Away a Dog from Her Chickens. Fig. 12
- Swan Driving Away an Intruder. Fig 13
- Head of Snarling Dog. Fig 14
- Cat Terrified at a Dog. Fig.15
- Cynopithecus Niger, Pleased by Being Caressed. Fig.17
- Chimpanzee Disappointed and Sulky. Fig. 18
- Screaming Infants. Plate I.
- Obliquity of the Eyebrows. Plate II
- Moderate Laughter and Smiling. Plate III
- Ill-temper. Plate IV
- Anger and Indignation. Plate VI
- Scorn and Disdain. Plate V
- Gestures of the Body. Plate VII
- Photograph of an Insane Woman. Fig. 19
- Terror. Fig. 20
- Horror and Agony. Fig. 21
- _N.B_.—Several of the figures in these seven Heliotype Plates have been
- reproduced from photographs, instead of from the original negatives;
- and they are in consequence somewhat indistinct. Nevertheless they are
- faithful copies, and are much superior for my purpose to any drawing,
- however carefully executed.
- DETAILED CONTENTS.
- INTRODUCTION
- CHAP. I—GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.
- The three chief principles stated—The first principle—Serviceable
- actions become habitual in association with certain states of the mind,
- and are performed whether or not of service in each particular case—The
- force of habit—Inheritance—Associated habitual movements in man—Reflex
- actions—Passage of habits into reflex actions—Associated habitual
- movements in the lower animals—Concluding remarks
- CHAP. II—GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.—_continued_.
- The Principle of Antithesis—Instances in the dog and cat—Origin of the
- principle—Conventional signs—The principle of antithesis has not arisen
- from opposite actions being consciously performed under opposite
- impulses
- CHAP. III—GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.—_concluded_.
- The principle of the direct action of the excited nervous system on the
- body, independently of the will and in part of habit—Change of colour
- in the hair—Trembling of the muscles—Modified
- secretions—Perspiration—Expression of extreme pain—Of rage, great joy,
- and terror—Contrast between the emotions which cause and do not cause
- expressive movements—Exciting and depressing states of the mind—Summary
- CHAP. IV—MEANS OF EXPRESSION. IN ANIMALS.
- The emission of sounds—Vocal sounds—Sounds otherwise produced—Erection
- of the dermal appendages, hairs, feathers, &c., under the emotions of
- anger and terror—The drawing back of the ears as a preparation for
- fighting, and as an expression of anger—Erection of the ears and
- raising the head, a sign of attention
- CHAP. V.—SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS.
- The Dog, various expressive movements of—Cats—Horses—Ruminants—Monkeys,
- their expression of joy and affection—Of pain—Anger Astonishment and
- Terror
- CHAP. VI.—SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING.
- The screaming and weeping of infants—Form of features—Age at which
- weeping commences—The effects of habitual restraint on
- weeping—Sobbing—Cause of the contraction of the muscles round the eyes
- during screaming—Cause of the secretion of tears
- CHAP. VII.—LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR.
- General effect of grief on the system—Obliquity of the eyebrows under
- suffering—On the cause of the obliquity of the eyebrows—On the
- depression of the corners of the mouth
- CHAP. VIII.—JOY, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION.
- Laughter primarily the expression of joy—Ludicrous ideas—Movements of
- the features during laughter—Nature of the sound produced—The secretion
- of tears during loud laughter—Gradation from loud laughter to gentle
- smiling—High spirits—The expression of love—Tender feelings—Devotion
- CHAP. IX.—REFLECTION—MEDITATION—ILL—TEMPER—SULKINESS DETERMINATION.
- The act of frowning—Reflection with an effort or with the perception of
- something difficult or disagreeable—Abstracted
- meditation—Ill-temper—Moroseness—Obstinacy—Sulkiness and
- pouting—Decision or determination—The firm closure of the mouth
- CHAP. X.—HATRED AND ANGER.
- Hatred—Rage, effects of on the system—Uncovering of the teeth—Rage in
- the insane—Anger and indignation—As expressed by the various races of
- man—Sneering and defiance—The uncovering of the canine teeth on one
- side of the face
- CHAP. XI.—DISDAIN—CONTEMPT—DISGUST—GUILT—PRIDE,
- ETC.—HELPLESSNESS—PATIENCE—AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION.
- Contempt, scorn and disdain, variously expressed—Derisive
- Smile—Gestures expressive of contempt—Disgust—Guilt, deceit, pride,
- etc.—Helplessness or impotence—Patience—Obstinacy—Shrugging the
- shoulders common to most of the races of man—Signs of affirmation and
- negation
- CHAP. XII.—SURPRISE—ASTONISHMENT—FEAR—HORROR.
- Surprise, astonishment—Elevation of the eyebrows—Opening the
- mouth—Protrusion of the lips—Gestures accompanying surprise—Admiration
- Fear—Terror—Erection of the hair—Contraction of the platysma
- muscle—Dilatation of the pupils—horror—Conclusion.
- CHAP. XIII.—SELF-ATTENTION—SHAME—SHYNESS—MODESTY: BLUSHING.
- Nature of a blush—Inheritance—The parts of the body most
- affected—Blushing in the various races of man—Accompanying
- gestures—Confusion of mind—Causes of blushing—Self-attention, the
- fundamental element—Shyness—Shame, from broken moral laws and
- conventional rules—Modesty—Theory of blushing—Recapitulation
- CHAP. XIV.—CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY.
- The three leading principles which have determined the chief movements
- of expression—Their inheritance—On the part which the will and
- intention have played in the acquirement of various expressions—The
- instinctive recognition of expression—The bearing of our subject on the
- specific unity of the races of man—On the successive acquirement of
- various expressions by the progenitors of man—The importance of
- expression—Conclusion
- ON THE EXPRESSION OF THE EMOTIONS IN MAN AND ANIMALS.
- INTRODUCTION.
- Many works have been written on Expression, but a greater number on
- Physiognomy,—that is, on the recognition of character through the study
- of the permanent form of the features. With this latter subject I am
- not here concerned. The older treatises,[1] which I have consulted,
- have been of little or no service to me. The famous ‘Conférences’[2] of
- the painter Le Brun, published in 1667, is the best known ancient work,
- and contains some good remarks. Another somewhat old essay, namely, the
- ‘Discours,’ delivered 1774-1782, by the well-known Dutch anatomist
- Camper,[3] can hardly be considered as having made any marked advance
- in the subject. The following works, on the contrary, deserve the
- fullest consideration.
- Sir Charles Bell, so illustrious for his discoveries in physiology,
- published in 1806 the first edition, and in the third edition of his
- ‘Anatomy and Philosophy of Expression.’[4] He may with justice be said,
- not only to have laid the foundations of the subject as a branch of
- science, but to have built up a noble structure. His work is in every
- way deeply interesting; it includes graphic descriptions of the various
- emotions, and is admirably illustrated. It is generally admitted that
- his service consists chiefly in having shown the intimate relation
- which exists between the movements of expression and those of
- respiration. One of the most important points, small as it may at first
- appear, is that the muscles round the eyes are involuntarily contracted
- during violent expiratory efforts, in order to protect these delicate
- organs from the pressure of the blood. This fact, which has been fully
- investigated for me with the greatest kindness by Professors Donders of
- Utrecht, throws, as we shall hereafter see, a flood of light on several
- of the most important expressions of the human countenance. The merits
- of Sir C. Bell’s work have been undervalued or quite ignored by several
- foreign writers, but have been fully admitted by some, for instance by
- M. Lemoine,[5] who with great justice says:—“Le livre de Ch. Bell
- devrait être médité par quiconque essaye de faire parler le visage de
- l’homme, par les philosophes aussi bien que par les artistes, car, sous
- une apparence plus légère et sous le prétexte de l’esthétique, c’est un
- des plus beaux monuments de la science des rapports du physique et du
- moral.”
- From reasons which will presently be assigned, Sir C. Bell did not
- attempt to follow out his views as far as they might have been carried.
- He does not try to explain why different muscles are brought into
- action under different emotions; why, for instance, the inner ends of
- the eyebrows are raised, and the corners of the mouth depressed, by a
- person suffering from grief or anxiety.
- In 1807 M. Moreau edited an edition of Lavater on Physiognomy,[6] in
- which he incorporated several of his own essays, containing excellent
- descriptions of the movements of the facial muscles, together with many
- valuable remarks. He throws, however, very little light on the
- philosophy of the subject. For instance, M. Moreau, in speaking of the
- act of frowning, that is, of the contraction of the muscle called by
- French writers the _soucilier_ (_corrigator supercilii_), remarks with
- truth:—“Cette action des sourciliers est un des symptômes les plus
- tranchés de l’expression des affections pénibles ou concentrées.” He
- then adds that these muscles, from their attachment and position, are
- fitted “à resserrer, à concentrer les principaux traits de la _face_,
- comme il convient dans toutes ces passions vraiment oppressives ou
- profondes, dans ces affections dont le sentiment semble porter
- l’organisation à revenir sur elle-même, à se contracter et à
- _s’amoindrir_, comme pour offrir moins de prise et de surface à des
- impressions redoutables ou importunes.” He who thinks that remarks of
- this kind throw any light on the meaning or origin of the different
- expressions, takes a very different view of the subject to what I do.
- In the above passage there is but a slight, if any, advance in the
- philosophy of the subject, beyond that reached by the painter Le Brun,
- who, in 1667, in describing the expression of fright, says:—“Le sourcil
- qui est abaissé d’un côté et élevé de l’autre, fait voir que la partie
- élevée semble le vouloir joindre au cerveau pour le garantir du mal que
- l’âme aperçoit, et le côté qui est abaissé et qui paraît enflé,—nous
- fait trouver dans cet état par les esprits qui viennent du cerveau en
- abondance, comme polir couvrir l’âme et la défendre du mal qu’elle
- craint; la bouche fort ouverte fait voir le saisissement du cœur, par
- le sang qui se retire vers lui, ce qui l’oblige, voulant respirer, à
- faire un effort qui est cause que la bouche s’ouvre extrêmement, et
- qui, lorsqu’il passe par les organes de la voix, forme un son qui n’est
- point articulé; que si les muscles et les veines paraissent enflés, ce
- n’est que par les esprits que le cerveau envoie en ces parties-là.” I
- have thought the foregoing sentences worth quoting, as specimens of the
- surprising nonsense which has been written on the subject.
- ‘The Physiology or Mechanism of Blushing,’ by Dr. Burgess, appeared in
- 1839, and to this work I shall frequently refer in my thirteenth
- Chapter.
- In 1862 Dr. Duchenne published two editions, in folio and octavo, of
- his ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ in which he analyses by
- means of electricity, and illustrates by magnificent photographs, the
- movements of the facial muscles. He has generously permitted me to copy
- as many of his photographs as I desired. His works have been spoken
- lightly of, or quite passed over, by some of his countrymen. It is
- possible that Dr. Duchenne may have exaggerated the importance of the
- contraction of single muscles in giving expression; for, owing to the
- intimate manner in which the muscles are connected, as may be seen in
- Henle’s anatomical drawings[7]—the best I believe ever published it is
- difficult to believe in their separate action. Nevertheless, it is
- manifest that Dr. Duchenne clearly apprehended this and other sources
- of error, and as it is known that he was eminently successful in
- elucidating the physiology of the muscles of the hand by the aid of
- electricity, it is probable that he is generally in the right about the
- muscles of the face. In my opinion, Dr. Duchenne has greatly advanced
- the subject by his treatment of it. No one has more carefully studied
- the contraction of each separate muscle, and the consequent furrows
- produced on the skin. He has also, and this is a very important
- service, shown which muscles are least under the separate control of
- the will. He enters very little into theoretical considerations, and
- seldom attempts to explain why certain muscles and not others contract
- under the influence of certain emotions.
- A distinguished French anatomist, Pierre Gratiolet, gave a course of
- lectures on Expression at the Sorbonne, and his notes were published
- (1865) after his death, under the title of ‘De la Physionomie et des
- Mouvements d’Expression.’ This is a very interesting work, full of
- valuable observations. His theory is rather complex, and, as far as it
- can be given in a single sentence (p. 65), is as follows:—“Il résulte,
- de tous les faits que j’ai rappelés, que les sens, l’imagination et la
- pensée elle-même, si élevée, si abstraite qu’on la suppose, ne peuvent
- s’exercer sans éveiller un sentiment corrélatif, et que ce sentiment se
- traduit directement, sympathiquement, symboliquement ou
- métaphoriquement, dans toutes les sphères des organs extérieurs, qui la
- racontent tous, suivant leur mode d’action propre, comme si chacun
- d’eux avait été directement affecté.”
- Gratiolet appears to overlook inherited habit, and even to some extent
- habit in the individual; and therefore he fails, as it seems to me, to
- give the right explanation, or any explanation at all, of many gestures
- and expressions. As an illustration of what he calls symbolic
- movements, I will quote his remarks (p. 37), taken from M. Chevreul, on
- a man playing at billiards. “Si une bille dévie légèrement de la
- direction que le joueur prétend lui imprimer, ne l’avez-vous pas vu
- cent fois la pousser du regard, de la tête et même des épaules, comme
- si ces mouvements, purement symboliques, pouvaient rectifier son
- trajet? Des mouvements non moins significatifs se produisent quand la
- bille manque d’une impulsion suffisante. Et cliez les joueurs novices,
- ils sont quelquefois accusés au point d’éveiller le sourire sur les
- lèvres des spectateurs.” Such movements, as it appeirs to me, may be
- attributed simply to habit. As often as a man has wished to move an
- object to one side, he has always pushed it to that side when forwards,
- he has pushed it forwards; and if he has wished to arrest it, he has
- pulled backwards. Therefore, when a man sees his ball travelling in a
- wrong direction, and he intensely wishes it to go in another direction,
- he cannot avoid, from long habit, unconsciously performing movements
- which in other cases he has found effectual.
- As an instance of sympathetic movements Gratiolet gives (p. 212) the
- following case:—“un jeune chien à oreilles droites, auquel son maître
- présente de loin quelque viande appétissante, fixe avec ardeur ses yeux
- sur cet objet dont il suit tous les mouvements, et pendant que les yeux
- regardent, les deux oreilles se portent en avant comme si cet objet
- pouvait être entendu.” Here, instead of speaking of sympathy between
- the ears and eyes, it appears to me more simple to believe, that as
- dogs during many generations have, whilst intently looking at any
- object, pricked their ears in order to perceive any sound; and
- conversely have looked intently in the direction of a sound to which
- they may have listened, the movements of these organs have become
- firmly associated together through long-continued habit.
- Dr. Piderit published in 1859 an essay on Expression, which I have not
- seen, but in which, as he states, he forestalled Gratiolet in many of
- his views. In 1867 he published his ‘Wissenschaftliches System der
- Mimik und Physiognomik.’ It is hardly possible to give in a few
- sentences a fair notion of his views; perhaps the two following
- sentences will tell as much as can be briefly told: “the muscular
- movements of expression are in part related to imaginary objects, and
- in part to imaginary sensorial impressions. In this proposition lies
- the key to the comprehension of all expressive muscular movements.” (s.
- 25) Again, “Expressive movements manifest themselves chiefly in the
- numerous and mobile muscles of the face, partly because the nerves by
- which they are set into motion originate in the most immediate vicinity
- of the mind-organ, but partly also because these muscles serve to
- support the organs of sense.” (s. 26.) If Dr. Piderit had studied Sir
- C. Bell’s work, he would probably not have said (s. 101) that violent
- laughter causes a frown from partaking of the nature of pain; or that
- with infants (s. 103) the tears irritate the eyes, and thus excite the
- contraction of the surrounding in muscles. Many good remarks are
- scattered throughout this volume, to which I shall hereafter refer.
- Short discussions on Expression may be found in various works, which
- need not here be particularised. Mr. Bain, however, in two of his works
- has treated the subject at some length. He says,[8] “I look upon the
- expression so-called as part and parcel of the feeling. I believe it to
- be a general law of the mind that along with the fact of inward feeling
- or consciousness, there is a diffusive action or excitement over the
- bodily members.” In another place he adds, “A very considerable number
- of the facts may be brought under the following principle: namely, that
- states of pleasure are connected with an increase, and states of pain
- with an abatement, of some, or all, of the vital functions.” But the
- above law of the diffusive action of feelings seems too general to
- throw much light on special expressions.
- Mr. Herbert Spencer, in treating of the Feelings in his ‘Principles of
- Psychology’ (1855), makes the following remarks:—“Fear, when strong,
- expresses itself in cries, in efforts to hide or escape, in
- palpitations and tremblings; and these are just the manifestations that
- would accompany an actual experience of the evil feared. The
- destructive passions are shown in a general tension of the muscular
- system, in gnashing of the teeth and protrusion of the claws, in
- dilated eyes and nostrils in growls; and these are weaker forms of the
- actions that accompany the killing of prey.” Here we have, as I
- believe, the true theory of a large number of expressions; but the
- chief interest and difficulty of the subject lies in following out the
- wonderfully complex results. I infer that some one (but who he is I
- have not been able to ascertain) formerly advanced a nearly similar
- view, for Sir C. Bell says,[9] “It has been maintained that what are
- called the external signs of passion, are only the concomitants of
- those voluntary movements which the structure renders necessary.” Mr.
- Spencer has also published[10] a valuable essay on the physiology of
- Laughter, in which he insists on “the general law that feeling passing
- a certain pitch, habitually vents itself in bodily action,” and that
- “an overflow of nerve-force undirected by any motive, will manifestly
- take first the most habitual routes; and if these do not suffice, will
- next overflow into the less habitual ones.” This law I believe to be of
- the highest importance in throwing light on our subject.’[11]
- All the authors who have written on Expression, with the exception of
- Mr. Spencer—the great expounder of the principle of Evolution—appear to
- have been firmly convinced that species, man of course included, came
- into existence in their present condition. Sir C. Bell, being thus
- convinced, maintains that many of our facial muscles are “purely
- instrumental in expression;” or are “a special provision” for this sole
- object.[12] But the simple fact that the anthropoid apes possess the
- same facial muscles as we do,[13] renders it very improbable that these
- muscles in our case serve exclusively for expression; for no one, I
- presume, would be inclined to admit that monkeys have been endowed with
- special muscles solely for exhibiting their hideous grimaces. Distinct
- uses, independently of expression, can indeed be assigned with much
- probability for almost all the facial muscles.
- Sir C. Bell evidently wished to draw as broad a distinction as possible
- between man and the lower animals; and he consequently asserts that
- with “the lower creatures there is no expression but what may be
- referred, more or less plainly, to their acts of volition or necessary
- instincts.” He further maintains that their faces “seem chiefly capable
- of expressing rage and fear.”[14] But man himself cannot express love
- and humility by external signs, so plainly as does a dog, when with
- drooping ears, hanging lips, flexuous body, and wagging tail, he meets
- his beloved master. Nor can these movements in the dog be explained by
- acts of volition or necessary instincts, any more than the beaming eyes
- and smiling cheeks of a man when he meets an old friend. If Sir C. Bell
- had been questioned about the expression of affection in the dog, he
- would no doubt have answered that this animal had been created with
- special instincts, adapting him for association with man, and that all
- further enquiry on the subject was superfluous.
- Although Gratiolet emphatically denies[15] that any muscle has been
- developed solely for the sake of expression, he seems never to have
- reflected on the principle of evolution. He apparently looks at each
- species as a separate creation. So it is with the other writers on
- Expression. For instance, Dr. Duchenne, after speaking of the movements
- of the limbs, refers to those which give expression to the face, and
- remarks:[16] “Le créateur n’a donc pas eu à se préoccuper ici des
- besoins de la mécanique; il a pu, selon sa sagesse, ou—que l’on me
- pardonne cette manière de parler—par une divine fantaisie, mettre en
- action tel ou tel muscle, un seul ou plusieurs muscles à la fois,
- lorsqu’il a voulu que les signes caractéristiques des passions, même
- les plus fugaces, fussent écrits passagèrement sur la face de l’homme.
- Ce langage de la physionomie une fois créé, il lui a suffi, pour le
- rendre universel et immuable, de donner à tout être humain la faculté
- instinctive d’exprimer toujours ses sendments par la contraction des
- mêmes muscles.”
- Many writers consider the whole subject of Expression as inexplicable.
- Thus the illustrious physiologist Müller, says,[17] “The completely
- different expression of the features in different passions shows that,
- according to the kind of feeling excited, entirely different groups of
- the fibres of the facial nerve are acted on. Of the cause of this we
- are quite ignorant.”
- No doubt as long as man and all other animals are viewed as independent
- creations, an effectual stop is put to our natural desire to
- investigate as far as possible the causes of Expression. By this
- doctrine, anything and everything can be equally well explained; and it
- has proved as pernicious with respect to Expression as to every other
- branch of natural history. With mankind some expressions, such as the
- bristling of the hair under the influence of extreme terror, or the
- uncovering of the teeth under that of furious rage, can hardly be
- understood, except on the belief that man once existed in a much lower
- and animal-like condition. The community of certain expressions in
- distinct though allied species, as in the movements of the same facial
- muscles during laughter by man and by various monkeys, is rendered
- somewhat more intelligible, if we believe in their descent from a
- common progenitor. He who admits on general grounds that the structure
- and habits of all animals have been gradually evolved, will look at the
- whole subject of Expression in a new and interesting light.
- The study of Expression is difficult, owing to the movements being
- often extremely slight, and of a fleeting nature. A difference may be
- clearly perceived, and yet it may be impossible, at least I have found
- it so, to state in what the difference consists. When we witness any
- deep emotion, our sympathy is so strongly excited, that close
- observation is forgotten or rendered almost impossible; of which fact I
- have had many curious proofs. Our imagination is another and still more
- serious source of error; for if from the nature of the circumstances we
- expect to see any expression, we readily imagine its presence.
- Notwithstanding Dr. Duchenne’s great experience, he for a long time
- fancied, as he states, that several muscles contracted under certain
- emotions, whereas he ultimately convinced himself that the movement was
- confined to a single muscle.
- In order to acquire as good a foundation as possible, and to ascertain,
- independently of common opinion, how far particular movements of the
- features and gestures are really expressive of certain states of the
- mind, I have found the following means the most serviceable. In the
- first place, to observe infants; for they exhibit many emotions, as Sir
- C. Bell remarks, “with extraordinary force;” whereas, in after life,
- some of our expressions “cease to have the pure and simple source from
- which they spring in infancy.”[18]
- In the second place, it occurred to me that the insane ought to be
- studied, as they are liable to the strongest passions, and give
- uncontrolled vent to them. I had, myself, no opportunity of doing this,
- so I applied to Dr. Maudsley and received from him an introduction to
- Dr. J. Crichton Browne, who has charge of an immense asylum near
- Wakefield, and who, as I found, had already attended to the subject.
- This excellent observer has with unwearied kindness sent me copious
- notes and descriptions, with valuable suggestions on many points; and I
- can hardly over-estimate the value of his assistance. I owe also, to
- the kindness of Mr. Patrick Nicol, of the Sussex Lunatic Asylum,
- interesting statements on two or three points.
- Thirdly Dr. Duchenne galvanized, as we have already seen, certain
- muscles in the face of an old man, whose skin was little sensitive, and
- thus produced various expressions which were photographed on a large
- scale. It fortunately occurred to me to show several of the best
- plates, without a word of explanation, to above twenty educated persons
- of various ages and both sexes, asking them, in each case, by what
- emotion or feeling the old man was supposed to be agitated; and I
- recorded their answers in the words which they used. Several of the
- expressions were instantly recognised by almost everyone, though
- described in not exactly the same terms; and these may, I think, be
- relied on as truthful, and will hereafter be specified. On the other
- hand, the most widely different judgments were pronounced in regard to
- some of them. This exhibition was of use in another way, by convincing
- me how easily we may be misguided by our imagination; for when I first
- looked through Dr. Duchenne’s photographs, reading at the same time the
- text, and thus learning what was intended, I was struck with admiration
- at the truthfulness of all, with only a few exceptions. Nevertheless,
- if I had examined them without any explanation, no doubt I should have
- been as much perplexed, in some cases, as other persons have been.
- Fourthly, I had hoped to derive much aid from the great masters in
- painting and sculpture, who are such close observers. Accordingly, I
- have looked at photographs and engravings of many well-known works;
- but, with a few exceptions, have not thus profited. The reason no doubt
- is, that in works of art, beauty is the chief object; and strongly
- contracted facial muscles destroy beauty.[19] The story of the
- composition is generally told with wonderful force and truth by
- skilfully given accessories.
- Fifthly, it seemed to me highly important to ascertain whether the same
- expressions and gestures prevail, as has often been asserted without
- much evidence, with all the races of mankind, especially with those who
- have associated but little with Europeans. Whenever the same movements
- of the features or body express the same emotions in several distinct
- races of man, we may infer with much probability, that such expressions
- are true ones,—that is, are innate or instinctive. Conventional
- expressions or gestures, acquired by the individual during early life,
- would probably have differed in the different races, in the same manner
- as do their languages. Accordingly I circulated, early in the year
- 1867, the following printed queries with a request, which has been
- fully responded to, that actual observations, and not memory, might be
- trusted. These queries were written after a considerable interval of
- time, during which my attention had been otherwise directed, and I can
- now see that they might have been greatly improved. To some of the
- later copies, I appended, in manuscript, a few additional remarks:—
- (1.) Is astonishment expressed by the eyes and mouth being opened wide,
- and by the eyebrows being raised?
- (2.) Does shame excite a blush when the colour of the skin allows it to
- be visible? and especially how low down the body does the blush extend?
- (3.) When a man is indignant or defiant does he frown, hold his body
- and head erect, square his shoulders and clench his fists?
- (4) When considering deeply on any subject, or trying to understand any
- puzzle, does he frown, or wrinkle the skin beneath the lower eyelids?
- (5.) When in low spirits, are the corners of the mouth depressed, and
- the inner corner of the eyebrows raised by that muscle which the French
- call the “Grief muscle”? The eyebrow in this state becomes slightly
- oblique, with a little swelling at the Inner end; and the forehead is
- transversely wrinkled in the middle part, but not across the whole
- breadth, as when the eyebrows are raised in surprise.
- (6.) When in good spirits do the eyes sparkle, with the skin a little
- wrinkled round and under them, and with the mouth a little drawn back
- at the corners?
- (7.) When a man sneers or snarls at another, is the corner of the upper
- lip over the canine or eye tooth raised on the side facing the man whom
- he addresses?
- (8) Can a dogged or obstinate expression be recognized, which is
- chiefly shown by the mouth being firmly closed, a lowering brow and a
- slight frown?
- (9.) Is contempt expressed by a slight protrusion of the lips and by
- turning up the nose, and with a slight expiration?
- (10) Is disgust shown by the lower lip being turned down, the upper lip
- slightly raised, with a sudden expiration, something like incipient
- vomiting, or like something spit out of the mouth?
- (11.) Is extreme fear expressed in the same general manner as with
- Europeans?
- (12.) Is laughter ever carried to such an extreme as to bring tears
- into the eyes?
- (13.) When a man wishes to show that he cannot prevent something being
- done, or cannot himself do something, does he shrug his shoulders, turn
- inwards his elbows, extend outwards his hands and open the palms; with
- the eyebrows raised?
- (14) Do the children when sulky, pout or greatly protrude the lips?
- (15.) Can guilty, or sly, or jealous expressions be recognized? though
- I know not how these can be defined.
- (16.) Is the head nodded vertically in affirmation, and shaken
- laterally in negation?
- Observations on natives who have had little communication with
- Europeans would be of course the most valuable, though those made on
- any natives would be of much interest to me. General remarks on
- expression are of comparatively little value; and memory is so
- deceptive that I earnestly beg it may not be trusted. A definite
- description of the countenance under any emotion or frame of mind, with
- a statement of the circumstances under which it occurred, would possess
- much value.
- To these queries I have received thirty-six answers from different
- observers, several of them missionaries or protectors of the
- aborigines, to all of whom I am deeply indebted for the great trouble
- which they have taken, and for the valuable aid thus received. I will
- specify their names, &c., towards the close of this chapter, so as not
- to interrupt my present remarks. The answers relate to several of the
- most distinct and savage races of man. In many instances, the
- circumstances have been recorded under which each expression was
- observed, and the expression itself described. In such cases, much
- confidence may be placed in the answers. When the answers have been
- simply yes or no, I have always received them with caution. It follows,
- from the information thus acquired, that the same state of mind is
- expressed throughout the world with remarkable uniformity; and this
- fact is in itself interesting as evidence of the close similarity in
- bodily structure and mental disposition of all the races, of mankind.
- Sixthly, and lastly, I have attended as closely as I could, to the
- expression of the several passions in some of the commoner animals; and
- this I believe to be of paramount importance, not of course for
- deciding how far in man certain expressions are characteristic of
- certain states of mind, but as affording the safest basis for
- generalisation on the causes, or origin, of the various movements of
- Expression. In observing animals, we are not so likely to be biassed by
- our imagination; and we may feel safe that their expressions are not
- conventional.
- From the reasons above assigned, namely, the fleeting nature of some
- expressions (the changes in the features being often extremely slight);
- our sympathy being easily aroused when we behold any strong emotion,
- and our attention thus distracted; our imagination deceiving us, from
- knowing in a vague manner what to expect, though certainly few of us
- know what the exact changes in the countenance are; and lastly, even
- our long familiarity with the subject,—from all these causes combined,
- the observation of Expression is by no means easy, as many persons,
- whom I have asked to observe certain points, have soon discovered.
- Hence it is difficult to determine, with certainty, what are the
- movements of the features and of the body, which commonly characterize
- certain states of the mind. Nevertheless, some of the doubts and
- difficulties have, as I hope, been cleared away by the observation of
- infants,—of the insane,—of the different races of man,—of works of
- art,—and lastly, of the facial muscles under the action of galvanism,
- as effected by Dr. Duchenne.
- But there remains the much greater difficulty of understanding the
- cause or origin of the several expressions, and of judging whether any
- theoretical explanation is trustworthy. Besides, judging as well as we
- can by our reason, without the aid of any rules, which of two or more
- explanations is the most satisfactory, or are quite unsatisfactory, I
- see only one way of testing our conclusions. This is to observe whether
- the same principle by which one expression can, as it appears, be
- explained, is applicable in other allied cases; and especially, whether
- the same general principles can be applied with satisfactory results,
- both to man and the lower animals. This latter method, I am inclined to
- think, is the most serviceable of all. The difficulty of judging of the
- truth of any theoretical explanation, and of testing it by some
- distinct line of investigation, is the great drawback to that interest
- which the study seems well fitted to excite.
- Finally, with respect to my own observations, I may state that they
- were commenced in the year 1838; and from that time to the present day,
- I have occasionally attended to the subject. At the above date, I was
- already inclined to believe in the principle of evolution, or of the
- derivation of species from other and lower forms. Consequently, when I
- read Sir C. Bell’s great work, his view, that man had been created with
- certain muscles specially adapted for the expression of his feelings,
- struck me as unsatisfactory. It seemed probable that the habit of
- expressing our feelings by certain movements, though now rendered
- innate, had been in some manner gradually acquired. But to discover how
- such habits had been acquired was perplexing in no small degree. The
- whole subject had to be viewed under a new aspect, and each expression
- demanded a rational explanation. This belief led me to attempt the
- present work, however imperfectly it may have been executed.
- I will now give the names of the gentlemen to whom, as I have said, I
- am deeply indebted for information in regard to the expressions
- exhibited by various races of man, and I will specify some of the
- circumstances under which the observations were in each case made.
- Owing to the great kindness and powerful influence of Mr. Wilson, of
- Hayes Place, Kent, I have received from Australia no less than thirteen
- sets of answers to my queries. This has been particularly fortunate, as
- the Australian aborigines rank amongst the most distinct of all the
- races of man. It will be seen that the observations have been chiefly
- made in the south, in the outlying parts of the colony of Victoria; but
- some excellent answers have been received from the north.
- Mr. Dyson Lacy has given me in detail some valuable observations, made
- several hundred miles in the interior of Queensland. To Mr. R. Brough
- Smyth, of Melbourne, I am much indebted for observations made by
- himself, and for sending me several of the following letters,
- namely:—From the Rev. Mr. Hagenauer, of Lake Wellington, a missionary
- in Gippsland, Victoria, who has had much experience with the natives.
- From Mr. Samuel Wilson, a landowner, residing at Langerenong, Wimmera,
- Victoria. From the Rev. George Taplin, superintendent of the native
- Industrial Settlement at Port Macleay. From Mr. Archibald G. Lang, of
- Coranderik, Victoria, a teacher at a school where aborigines, old and
- young, are collected from all parts of the colony. From Mr. H. B. Lane,
- of Belfast, Victoria, a police magistrate and warden, whose
- observations, as I am assured, are highly trustworthy. From Mr.
- Templeton Bunnett, of Echuca, whose station is on the borders of the
- colony of Victoria, and who has thus been able to observe many
- aborigines who have had little intercourse with white men. He compared
- his observations with those made by two other gentlemen long resident
- in the neighbourhood. Also from Mr. J. Bulmer, a missionary in a remote
- part of Gippsland, Victoria.
- I am also indebted to the distinguished botanist, Dr. Ferdinand Müller,
- of Victoria, for some observations made by himself, and for sending me
- others made by Mrs. Green, as well as for some of the foregoing
- letters.
- In regard to the Maoris of New Zealand, the Rev. J. W. Stack has
- answered only a few of my queries; but the answers have been remarkably
- full, clear, and distinct, with the circumstances recorded under which
- the observations were made.
- The Rajah Brooke has given me some information with respect to the
- Dyaks of Borneo.
- Respecting the Malays, I have been highly successful; for Mr. F. Geach
- (to whom I was introduced by Mr. Wallace), during his residence as a
- mining engineer in the interior of Malacca, observed many natives, who
- had never before associated with white men. He wrote me two long
- letters with admirable and detailed observations on their expression.
- He likewise observed the Chinese immigrants in the Malay archipelago.
- The well-known naturalist, H. M. Consul, Mr. Swinhoe, also observed for
- me the Chinese in their native country; and he made inquiries from
- others whom he could trust.
- In India Mr. H. Erskine, whilst residing in his official capacity in
- the Admednugur District in the Bombay Presidency, attended to the
- expression of the inhabitants, but found much difficulty in arriving at
- any safe conclusions, owing to their habitual concealment of all
- emotions in the presence of Europeans. He also obtained information for
- me from Mr. West, the Judge in Canara, and he consulted some
- intelligent native gentlemen on certain points. In Calcutta Mr. J.
- Scott, curator of the Botanic Gardens, carefully observed the various
- tribes of men therein employed during a considerable period, and no one
- has sent me such full and valuable details. The habit of accurate
- observation, gained by his botanical studies, has been brought to bear
- on our present subject. For Ceylon I am much indebted to the Rev. S. O.
- Glenie for answers to some of my queries.
- Turning to Africa, I have been unfortunate with respect to the negroes,
- though Mr. Winwood Reade aided me as far as lay in his power. It would
- have been comparatively easy to have obtained information in regard to
- the negro slaves in America; but as they have long associated with
- white men, such observations would have possessed little value. In the
- southern parts of the continent Mrs. Barber observed the Kafirs and
- Fingoes, and sent me many distinct answers. Mr. J. P. Mansel Weale also
- made some observations on the natives, and procured for me a curious
- document, namely, the opinion, written in English, of Christian Gaika,
- brother of the Chief Sandilli, on the expressions of his
- fellow-countrymen. In the northern regions of Africa Captain Speedy,
- who long resided with the Abyssinians, answered my queries partly from
- memory and partly from observations made on the son of King Theodore,
- who was then under his charge. Professor and Mrs. Asa Gray attended to
- some points in the expressions of the natives, as observed by them
- whilst ascending the Nile.
- On the great American continent Mr. Bridges, a catechist residing with
- the Fuegians, answered some few questions about their expression,
- addressed to him many years ago. In the northern half of the continent
- Dr. Rothrock attended to the expressions of the wild Atnah and Espyox
- tribes on the Nasse River, in North-Western America. Mr. Washington
- Matthews Assistant-Surgeon in the United States Army, also observed
- with special care (after having seen my queries, as printed in the
- ‘Smithsonian Report’) some of the wildest tribes in the Western parts
- of the United States, namely, the Tetons, Grosventres, Mandans, and
- Assinaboines; and his answers have proved of the highest value.
- Lastly, besides these special sources of information, I have collected
- some few facts incidentally given in books of travels.——
- Muscles of the Human Face. Fig 1-2
- Muscles of the Human Face. Fig 3
- As I shall often have to refer, more especially in the latter part of
- this volume, to the muscles of the human face, I have had a diagram
- (fig. 1) copied and reduced from Sir C. Bell’s work, and two others,
- with more accurate details (figs. 2 and 3), from Herde’s well-known
- ‘Handbuch der Systematischen Anatomie des Menschen.’ The same letters
- refer to the same muscles in all three figures, but the names are given
- of only the more important ones to which I shall have to allude. The
- facial muscles blend much together, and, as I am informed, hardly
- appear on a dissected face so distinct as they are here represented.
- Some writers consider that these muscles consist of nineteen pairs,
- with one unpaired;[20] but others make the number much larger,
- amounting even to fifty-five, according to Moreau. They are, as is
- admitted by everyone who has written on the subject, very variable in
- structure; and Moreau remarks that they are hardly alike in
- half-a-dozen subjects.[21] They are also variable in function. Thus the
- power of uncovering the canine tooth on one side differs much in
- different persons. The power of raising the wings of the nostrils is
- also, according to Dr. Piderit,[22] variable in a remarkable degree;
- and other such cases could be given.
- Finally, I must have the pleasure of expressing my obligations to Mr.
- Rejlander for the trouble which he has taken in photographing for me
- various expressions and gestures. I am also indebted to Herr
- Kindermann, of Hamburg, for the loan of some excellent negatives of
- crying infants; and to Dr. Wallich for a charming one of a smiling
- girl. I have already expressed my obligations to Dr. Duchenne for
- generously permitting me to have some of his large photographs copied
- and reduced. All these photographs have been printed by the Heliotype
- process, and the accuracy of the copy is thus guaranteed. These plates
- are referred to by Roman numerals.
- I am also greatly indebted to Mr. T. W. Wood for the extreme pains
- which he has taken in drawing from life the expressions of various
- animals. A distinguished artist, Mr. Riviere, has had the kindness to
- give me two drawings of dogs—one in a hostile and the other in a humble
- and caressing frame of mind. Mr. A. May has also given me two similar
- sketches of dogs. Mr. Cooper has taken much care in cutting the blocks.
- Some of the photographs and drawings, namely, those by Mr. May, and
- those by Mr. Wolf of the Cynopithecus, were first reproduced by Mr.
- Cooper on wood by means of photography, and then engraved: by this
- means almost complete fidelity is ensured.
- CHAPTER I. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION.
- The three chief principles stated—The first principle—Serviceable
- actions become habitual in association with certain states of the mind,
- and are performed whether or not of service in each particular case—The
- force of habit—Inheritance—Associated habitual movements in man—Reflex
- actions—Passage of habits into reflex actions—Associated habitual
- movements in the lower animals—Concluding remarks.
- I will begin by giving the three Principles, which appear to me to
- account for most of the expressions and gestures involuntarily used by
- man and the lower animals, under the influence of various emotions and
- sensations.[101] I arrived, however, at these three Principles only at
- the close of my observations. They will be discussed in the present and
- two following chapters in a general manner. Facts observed both with
- man and the lower animals will here be made use of; but the latter
- facts are preferable, as less likely to deceive us. In the fourth and
- fifth chapters, I will describe the special expressions of some of the
- lower animals; and in the succeeding chapters those of man. Everyone
- will thus be able to judge for himself, how far my three principles
- throw light on the theory of the subject. It appears to me that so many
- expressions are thus explained in a fairly satisfactory manner, that
- probably all will hereafter be found to come under the same or closely
- analogous heads. I need hardly premise that movements or changes in any
- part of the body,—as the wagging of a dog’s tail, the drawing back of a
- horse’s ears, the shrugging of a man’s shoulders, or the dilatation of
- the capillary vessels of the skin,—may all equally well serve for
- expression. The three Principles are as follows.
- I. _The principle of serviceable associated Habits_.—Certain complex
- actions are of direct or indirect service under certain states of the
- mind, in order to relieve or gratify certain sensations, desires, &c.;
- and whenever the same state of mind is induced, however feebly, there
- is a tendency through the force of habit and association for the same
- movements to be performed, though they may not then be of the least
- use. Some actions ordinarily associated through habit with certain
- states of the mind may be partially repressed through the will, and in
- such cases the muscles which are least under the separate control of
- the will are the most liable still to act, causing movements which we
- recognize as expressive. In certain other cases the checking of one
- habitual movement requires other slight movements; and these are
- likewise expressive.
- II. _The principle of Antithesis_.—Certain states of the mind lead to
- certain habitual actions, which are of service, as under our first
- principle. Now when a directly opposite state of mind is induced, there
- is a strong and involuntary tendency to the performance of movements of
- a directly opposite nature, though these are of no use; and such
- movements are in some cases highly expressive.
- III. _The principle of actions due to the constitution of the Nervous
- System, independently from the first of the Will, and independently to
- a certain extent of Habit_.—When the sensorium is strongly excited,
- nerve-force is generated in excess, and is transmitted in certain
- definite directions, depending on the connection of the nerve-cells,
- and partly on habit: or the supply of nerve-force may, as it appears,
- be interrupted. Effects are thus produced which we recognize as
- expressive. This third principle may, for the sake of brevity, be
- called that of the direct action of the nervous system.
- With respect to our _first Principle_, it is notorious how powerful is
- the force of habit. The most complex and difficult movements can in
- time be performed without the least effort or consciousness. It is not
- positively known how it comes that habit is so efficient in
- facilitating complex movements; but physiologists admit[102] “that the
- conducting power of the nervous fibres increases with the frequency of
- their excitement.” This applies to the nerves of motion and sensation,
- as well as to those connected with the act of thinking. That some
- physical change is produced in the nerve-cells or nerves which are
- habitually used can hardly be doubted, for otherwise it is impossible
- to understand how the tendency to certain acquired movements is
- inherited. That they are inherited we see with horses in certain
- transmitted paces, such as cantering and ambling, which are not natural
- to them,—in the pointing of young pointers and the setting of young
- setters—in the peculiar manner of flight of certain breeds of the
- pigeon, &c. We have analogous cases with mankind in the inheritance of
- tricks or unusual gestures, to which we shall presently recur. To those
- who admit the gradual evolution of species, a most striking instance of
- the perfection with which the most difficult consensual movements can
- be transmitted, is afforded by the humming-bird Sphinx-moth
- (_Macroglossa_); for this moth, shortly after its emergence from the
- cocoon, as shown by the bloom on its unruffled scales, may be seen
- poised stationary in the air, with its long hair-like proboscis
- uncurled and inserted into the minute orifices of flowers; and no one,
- I believe, has ever seen this moth learning to perform its difficult
- task, which requires such unerring aim.
- When there exists an inherited or instinctive tendency to the
- performance of an action, or an inherited taste for certain kinds of
- food, some degree of habit in the individual is often or generally
- requisite. We find this in the paces of the horse, and to a certain
- extent in the pointing of dogs; although some young dogs point
- excellently the first time they are taken out, yet they often associate
- the proper inherited attitude with a wrong odour, and even with
- eyesight. I have heard it asserted that if a calf be allowed to suck
- its mother only once, it is much more difficult afterwards to rear it
- by hand.[103] Caterpillars which have been fed on the leaves of one
- kind of tree, have been known to perish from hunger rather than to eat
- the leaves of another tree, although this afforded them their proper
- food, under a state of nature;[104] and so it is in many other cases.
- The power of Association is admitted by everyone. Mr. Bain remarks,
- that “actions, sensations and states of feeling, occurring together or
- in close succession, tend to grow together, or cohere, in such a way
- that when any one of them is afterwards presented to the mind, the
- others are apt to be brought up in idea.”[105] It is so important for
- our purpose fully to recognize that actions readily become associated
- with other actions and with various states of the mind, that I will
- give a good many instances, in the first place relating to man, and
- afterwards to the lower animals. Some of the instances are of a very
- trifling nature, but they are as good for our purpose as more important
- habits. It is known to everyone how difficult, or even impossible it
- is, without repeated trials, to move the limbs in certain opposed
- directions which have never been practised. Analogous cases occur with
- sensations, as in the common experiment of rolling a marble beneath the
- tips of two crossed fingers, when it feels exactly like two marbles.
- Everyone protects himself when falling to the ground by extending his
- arms, and as Professor Alison has remarked, few can resist acting thus,
- when voluntarily falling on a soft bed. A man when going out of doors
- puts on his gloves quite unconsciously; and this may seem an extremely
- simple operation, but he who has taught a child to put on gloves, knows
- that this is by no means the case.
- When our minds are much affected, so are the movements of our bodies;
- but here another principle besides habit, namely the undirected
- overflow of nerve-force, partially comes into play. Norfolk, in
- speaking of Cardinal Wolsey, says—
- “Some strange commotion
- Is in his brain; he bites his lip and starts;
- Stops on a sudden, looks upon the ground,
- Then, lays his finger on his temple: straight,
- Springs out into fast gait; then, stops again,
- Strikes his breast hard; and anon, he casts
- His eye against the moon: in most strange postures
- We have seen him set himself.”—_Hen. VIII_., act iii, sc. 2.
- A vulgar man often scratches his head when perplexed in mind; and I
- believe that he acts thus from habit, as if he experienced a slightly
- uncomfortable bodily sensation, namely, the itching of his head, to
- which he is particularly liable, and which he thus relieves. Another
- man rubs his eyes when perplexed, or gives a little cough when
- embarrassed, acting in either case as if he felt a slightly
- uncomfortable sensation in his eyes or windpipe.[106]
- From the continued use of the eyes, these organs are especially liable
- to be acted on through association under various states of the mind,
- although there is manifestly nothing to be seen. A man, as Gratiolet
- remarks, who vehemently rejects a proposition, will almost certainly
- shut his eyes or turn away his face; but if he accepts the proposition,
- he will nod his head in affirmation and open his eyes widely. The man
- acts in this latter case as if he clearly saw the thing, and in the
- former case as if he did not or would not see it. I have noticed that
- persons in describing a horrid sight often shut their eyes momentarily
- and firmly, or shake their heads, as if not to see or to drive away
- something disagreeable; and I have caught myself, when thinking in the
- dark of a horrid spectacle, closing my eyes firmly. In looking suddenly
- at any object, or in looking all around, everyone raises his eyebrows,
- so that the eyes may be quickly and widely opened; and Duchenne remarks
- that[107] a person in trying to remember something often raises his
- eyebrows, as if to see it. A Hindoo gentleman made exactly the same
- remark to Mr. Erskine in regard to his countrymen. I noticed a young
- lady earnestly trying to recollect a painter’s name, and she first
- looked to one corner of the ceiling and then to the opposite corner,
- arching the one eyebrow on that side; although, of course, there was
- nothing to be seen there.
- In most of the foregoing cases, we can understand how the associated
- movements were acquired through habit; but with some individuals,
- certain strange gestures or tricks have arisen in association with
- certain states of the mind, owing to wholly inexplicable causes, and
- are undoubtedly inherited. I have elsewhere given one instance from my
- own observation of an extraordinary and complex gesture, associated
- with pleasurable feelings, which was transmitted from a father to his
- daughter, as well as some other analogous facts.[108] Another curious
- instance of an odd inherited movement, associated with the wish to
- obtain an object, will be given in the course of this volume.
- There are other actions which are commonly performed under certain
- circumstances, independently of habit, and which seem to be due to
- imitation or some sort of sympathy. Thus persons cutting anything with
- a pair of scissors may be seen to move their jaws simultaneously with
- the blades of the scissors. Children learning to write often twist
- about their tongues as their fingers move, in a ridiculous fashion.
- When a public singer suddenly becomes a little hoarse, many of those
- present may be heard, as I have been assured by a gentleman on whom I
- can rely, to clear their throats; but here habit probably comes into
- play, as we clear our own throats under similar circumstances. I have
- also been told that at leaping matches, as the performer makes his
- spring, many of the spectators, generally men and boys, move their
- feet; but here again habit probably comes into play, for it is very
- doubtful whether women would thus act.
- _Reflex actions_—Reflex actions, in the strict sense of the term, are
- due to the excitement of a peripheral nerve, which transmits its
- influence to certain nerve-cells, and these in their turn excite
- certain muscles or glands into action; and all this may take place
- without any sensation or consciousness on our part, though often thus
- accompanied. As many reflex actions are highly expressive, the subject
- must here be noticed at some little length. We shall also see that some
- of them graduate into, and can hardly be distinguished from actions
- which have arisen through habit?[109] Coughing and sneezing are
- familiar instances of reflex actions. With infants the first act of
- respiration is often a sneeze, although this requires the co-ordinated
- movement of numerous muscles. Respiration is partly voluntary, but
- mainly reflex, and is performed in the most natural and best manner
- without the interference of the will. A vast number of complex
- movements are reflex. As good an instance as can be given is the
- often-quoted one of a decapitated frog, which cannot of course feel,
- and cannot consciously perform, any movement. Yet if a drop of acid be
- placed on the lower surface of the thigh of a frog in this state, it
- will rub off the drop with the upper surface of the foot of the same
- leg. If this foot be cut off, it cannot thus act. “After some fruitless
- efforts, therefore, it gives up trying in that way, seems restless, as
- though, says Pflüger, it was seeking some other way, and at last it
- makes use of the foot of the other leg and succeeds in rubbing off the
- acid. Notably we have here not merely contractions of muscles, but
- combined and harmonized contractions in due sequence for a special
- purpose. These are actions that have all the appearance of being guided
- by intelligence and instigated by will in an animal, the recognized
- organ of whose intelligence and will has been removed.”[110]
- We see the difference between reflex and voluntary movements in very
- young children not being able to perform, as I am informed by Sir Henry
- Holland, certain acts somewhat analogous to those of sneezing and
- coughing, namely, in their not being able to blow their noses (_i.e._
- to compress the nose and blow violently through the passage), and in
- their not being able to clear their throats of phlegm. They have to
- learn to perform these acts, yet they are performed by us, when a
- little older, almost as easily as reflex actions. Sneezing and
- coughing, however, can be controlled by the will only partially or not
- at all; whilst the clearing the throat and blowing the nose are
- completely under our command.
- When we are conscious of the presence of an irritating particle in our
- nostrils or windpipe—that is, when the same sensory nerve-cells are
- excited, as in the case of sneezing and coughing—we can voluntarily
- expel the particle by forcibly driving air through these passages; but
- we cannot do this with nearly the same force, rapidity, and precision,
- as by a reflex action. In this latter case the sensory nerve-cells
- apparently excite the motor nerve-cells without any waste of power by
- first communicating with the cerebral hemispheres—the seat of our
- consciousness and volition. In all cases there seems to exist a
- profound antagonism between the same movements, as directed by the will
- and by a reflex stimulant, in the force with which they are performed
- and in the facility with which they are excited. As Claude Bernard
- asserts, “L’influence du cerveau tend donc à entraver les mouvements
- réflexes, à limiter leur force et leur étendue.”[111]
- The conscious wish to perform a reflex action sometimes stops or
- interrupts its performance, though the proper sensory nerves may be
- stimulated. For instance, many years ago I laid a small wager with a
- dozen young men that they would not sneeze if they took snuff, although
- they all declared that they invariably did so; accordingly they all
- took a pinch, but from wishing much to succeed, not one sneezed, though
- their eyes watered, and all, without exception, had to pay me the
- wager. Sir H. Holland remarks[112] that attention paid to the act of
- swallowing interferes with the proper movements; from which it probably
- follows, at least in part, that some persons find it so difficult to
- swallow a pill.
- Another familiar instance of a reflex action is the involuntary closing
- of the eyelids when the surface of the eye is touched. A similar
- winking movement is caused when a blow is directed towards the face;
- but this is an habitual and not a strictly reflex action, as the
- stimulus is conveyed through the mind and not by the excitement of a
- peripheral nerve. The whole body and head are generally at the same
- time drawn suddenly backwards. These latter movements, however, can be
- prevented, if the danger does not appear to the imagination imminent;
- but our reason telling us that there is no danger does not suffice. I
- may mention a trifling fact, illustrating this point, and which at the
- time amused me. I put my face close to the thick glass-plate in front
- of a puff-adder in the Zoological Gardens, with the firm determination
- of not starting back if the snake struck at me; but, as soon as the
- blow was struck, my resolution went for nothing, and I jumped a yard or
- two backwards with astonishing rapidity. My will and reason were
- powerless against the imagination of a danger which had never been
- experienced.
- The violence of a start seems to depend partly on the vividness of the
- imagination, and partly on the condition, either habitual or temporary,
- of the nervous system. He who will attend to the starting of his horse,
- when tired and fresh, will perceive how perfect is the gradation from a
- mere glance at some unexpected object, with a momentary doubt whether
- it is dangerous, to a jump so rapid and violent, that the animal
- probably could not voluntarily whirl round in so rapid a manner. The
- nervous system of a fresh and highly-fed horse sends its order to the
- motory system so quickly, that no time is allowed for him to consider
- whether or not the danger is real. After one violent start, when he is
- excited and the blood flows freely through his brain, he is very apt to
- start again; and so it is, as I have noticed, with young infants.
- A start from a sudden noise, when the stimulus is conveyed through the
- auditory nerves, is always accompanied in grown-up persons by the
- winking of the eyelids.[113] I observed, however, that though my
- infants started at sudden sounds, when under a fortnight old, they
- certainly did not always wink their eyes, and I believe never did so.
- The start of an older infant apparently represents a vague catching
- hold of something to prevent falling. I shook a pasteboard box close
- before the eyes of one of my infants, when 114 days old, and it did not
- in the least wink; but when I put a few comfits into the box, holding
- it in the same position as before, and rattled them, the child blinked
- its eyes violently every time, and started a little. It was obviously
- impossible that a carefully-guarded infant could have learnt by
- experience that a rattling sound near its eyes indicated danger to
- them. But such experience will have been slowly gained at a later age
- during a long series of generations; and from what we know of
- inheritance, there is nothing improbable in the transmission of a habit
- to the offspring at an earlier age than that at which it was first
- acquired by the parents.
- From the foregoing remarks it seems probable that some actions, which
- were at first performed consciously, have become through habit and
- association converted into reflex actions, and are now so firmly fixed
- and inherited, that they are performed, even when not of the least
- use,[114] as often as the same causes arise, which originally excited
- them in us through the volition. In such cases the sensory nerve-cells
- excite the motor cells, without first communicating with those cells on
- which our consciousness and volition depend. It is probable that
- sneezing and coughing were originally acquired by the habit of
- expelling, as violently as possible, any irritating particle from the
- sensitive air-passages. As far as time is concerned, there has been
- more than enough for these habits to have become innate or converted
- into reflex actions; for they are common to most or all of the higher
- quadrupeds, and must therefore have been first acquired at a very
- remote period. Why the act of clearing the throat is not a reflex
- action, and has to be learnt by our children, I cannot pretend to say;
- but we can see why blowing the nose on a handkerchief has to be learnt.
- It is scarcely credible that the movements of a headless frog, when it
- wipes off a drop of acid or other object from its thigh, and which
- movements are so well coordinated for a special purpose, were not at
- first performed voluntarily, being afterwards rendered easy through
- long-continued habit so as at last to be performed unconsciously, or
- independently of the cerebral hemispheres.
- So again it appears probable that starting was originally acquired by
- the habit of jumping away as quickly as possible from danger, whenever
- any of our senses gave us warning. Starting, as we have seen, is
- accompanied by the blinking of the eyelids so as to protect the eyes,
- the most tender and sensitive organs of the body; and it is, I believe,
- always accompanied by a sudden and forcible inspiration, which is the
- natural preparation for any violent effort. But when a man or horse
- starts, his heart beats wildly against his ribs, and here it may be
- truly said we have an organ which has never been under the control of
- the will, partaking in the general reflex movements of the body. To
- this point, however, I shall return in a future chapter.
- The contraction of the iris, when the retina is stimulated by a bright
- light, is another instance of a movement, which it appears cannot
- possibly have been at first voluntarily performed and then fixed by
- habit; for the iris is not known to be under the conscious control of
- the will in any animal. In such cases some explanation, quite distinct
- from habit, will have to be discovered. The radiation of nerve-force
- from strongly-excited nerve-cells to other connected cells, as in the
- case of a bright light on the retina causing a sneeze, may perhaps aid
- us in understanding how some reflex actions originated. A radiation of
- nerve-force of this kind, if it caused a movement tending to lessen the
- primary irritation, as in the case of the contraction of the iris
- preventing too much light from falling on the retina, might afterwards
- have been taken advantage of and modified for this special purpose.
- It further deserves notice that reflex actions are in all probability
- liable to slight variations, as are all corporeal structures and
- instincts; and any variations which were beneficial and of sufficient
- importance, would tend to be preserved and inherited. Thus reflex
- actions, when once gained for one purpose, might afterwards be modified
- independently of the will or habit, so as to serve for some distinct
- purpose. Such cases would be parallel with those which, as we have
- every reason to believe, have occurred with many instincts; for
- although some instincts have been developed simply through
- long-continued and inherited habit, other highly complex ones have been
- developed through the preservation of variations of pre-existing
- instincts—that is, through natural selection.
- I have discussed at some little length, though as I am well aware, in a
- very imperfect manner, the acquirement of reflex actions, because they
- are often brought into play in connection with movements expressive of
- our emotions; and it was necessary to show that at least some of them
- might have been first acquired through the will in order to satisfy a
- desire, or to relieve a disagreeable sensation.
- _Associated habitual movements in the lower animals_.—I have already
- given in the case of Man several instances of movements associated with
- various states of the mind or body, which are now purposeless, but
- which were originally of use, and are still of use under certain
- circumstances. As this subject is very important for us, I will here
- give a considerable number of analogous facts, with reference to
- animals; although many of them are of a very trifling nature. My object
- is to show that certain movements were originally performed for a
- definite end, and that, under nearly the same circumstances, they are
- still pertinaciously performed through habit when not of the least use.
- That the tendency in most of the following cases is inherited, we may
- infer from such actions being performed in the same manner by all the
- individuals, young and old, of the same species. We shall also see that
- they are excited by the most diversified, often circuitous, and
- sometimes mistaken associations.
- Dogs, when they wish to go to sleep on a carpet or other hard surface,
- generally turn round and round and scratch the ground with their
- fore-paws in a senseless manner, as if they intended to trample down
- the grass and scoop out a hollow, as no doubt their wild parents did,
- when they lived on open grassy plains or in the woods. Jackals,
- fennecs, and other allied animals in the Zoological Gardens, treat
- their straw in this manner; but it is a rather odd circumstance that
- the keepers, after observing for some months, have never seen the
- wolves thus behave. A semi-idiotic dog—and an animal in this condition
- would be particularly liable to follow a senseless habit—was observed
- by a friend to turn completely round on a carpet thirteen times before
- going to sleep.
- Many carnivorous animals, as they crawl towards their prey and prepare
- to rush or spring on it, lower their heads and crouch, partly, as it
- would appear, to hide themselves, and partly to get ready for their
- rush; and this habit in an exaggerated form has become hereditary in
- our pointers and setters. Now I have noticed scores of times that when
- two strange dogs meet on an open road, the one which first sees the
- other, though at the distance of one or two hundred yards, after the
- first glance always lowers its bead, generally crouches a little, or
- even lies down; that is, he takes the proper attitude for concealing
- himself and for making a rush or spring although the road is quite open
- and the distance great. Again, dogs of all kinds when intently watching
- and slowly approaching their prey, frequently keep one of their
- fore-legs doubled up for a long time, ready for the next cautious step;
- and this is eminently characteristic of the pointer. But from habit
- they behave in exactly the same manner whenever their attention is
- aroused (fig. 4). I have seen a dog at the foot of a high wall,
- listening attentively to a sound on the opposite side, with one leg
- doubled up; and in this case there could have been no intention of
- making a cautious approach.
- Small Dog Watching a Cat on A Table. Figure 4
- {illust. caption = for making a rush or FIG. 4.—Small dog watching a
- cat on a table. From a photograph taken by Mr. Rejlander.}
- Dogs after voiding their excrement often make with all four feet a few
- scratches backwards, even on a bare stone pavement, as if for the
- purpose of covering up their excrement with earth, in nearly the same
- manner as do cats. Wolves and jackals behave in the Zoological Gardens
- in exactly the same manner, yet, as I am assured by the keepers,
- neither wolves, jackals, nor foxes, when they have the means of doing
- so, ever cover up their excrement, any more than do dogs. All these
- animals, however, bury superfluous food. Hence, if we rightly
- understand the meaning of the above cat-like habit, of which there can
- be little doubt, we have a purposeless remnant of an habitual movement,
- which was originally followed by some remote progenitor of the
- dog-genus for a definite purpose, and which has been retained for a
- prodigious length of time.
- Dogs and jackals[115] take much pleasure in rolling and rubbing their
- necks and backs on carrion. The odour seems delightful to them, though
- dogs at least do not eat carrion. Mr. Bartlett has observed wolves for
- me, and has given them carrion, but has never seen them roll on it. I
- have heard it remarked, and I believe it to be true, that the larger
- dogs, which are probably descended from wolves, do not so often roll in
- carrion as do smaller dogs, which are probably descended from jackals.
- When a piece of brown biscuit is offered to a terrier of mine and she
- is not hungry (and I have heard of similar instances), she first tosses
- it about and worries it, as if it were a rat or other prey; she then
- repeatedly rolls on it precisely as if it were a piece of carrion, and
- at last eats it. It would appear that an imaginary relish has to be
- given to the distasteful morsel; and to effect this the dog acts in his
- habitual manner, as if the biscuit was a live animal or smelt like
- carrion, though he knows better than we do that this is not the case. I
- have seen this same terrier act in the same manner after killing a
- little bird or mouse.
- Dogs scratch themselves by a rapid movement of one of their hind-feet;
- and when their backs are rubbed with a stick, so strong is the habit,
- that they cannot help rapidly scratching the air or the ground in a
- useless and ludicrous manner. The terrier just alluded to, when thus
- scratched with a stick, will sometimes show her delight by another
- habitual movement, namely, by licking the air as if it were my hand.
- Horses scratch themselves by nibbling those parts of their bodies which
- they can reach with their teeth; but more commonly one horse shows
- another where he wants to be scratched, and they then nibble each
- other. A friend whose attention I had called to the subject, observed
- that when he rubbed his horse’s neck, the animal protruded his head,
- uncovered his teeth, and moved his jaws, exactly as if nibbling another
- horse’s neck, for he could never have nibbled his own neck. If a horse
- is much tickled, as when curry-combed, his wish to bite something
- becomes so intolerably strong, that he will clatter his teeth together,
- and though not vicious, bite his groom. At the same time from habit he
- closely depresses his ears, so as to protect them from being bitten, as
- if he were fighting with another horse.
- A horse when eager to start on a journey makes the nearest approach
- which he can to the habitual movement of progression by pawing the
- ground. Now when horses in their stalls are about to be fed and are
- eager for their corn, they paw the pavement or the straw. Two of my
- horses thus behave when they see or hear the corn given to their
- neighbours. But here we have what may almost be called a true
- expression, as pawing the ground is universally recognized as a sign of
- eagerness.
- Cats cover up their excrements of both kinds with earth; and my
- grandfather[116] saw a kitten scraping ashes over a spoonful of pure
- water spilt on the hearth; so that here an habitual or instinctive
- action was falsely excited, not by a previous act or by odour, but by
- eyesight. It is well known that cats dislike wetting their feet, owing,
- it is probable, to their having aboriginally inhabited the dry country
- of Egypt; and when they wet their feet they shake them violently. My
- daughter poured some water into a glass close to the head of a kitten;
- and it immediately shook its feet in the usual manner; so that here we
- have an habitual movement falsely excited by an associated sound
- instead of by the sense of touch.
- Kittens, puppies, young pigs and probably many other young animals,
- alternately push with their forefeet against the mammary glands of
- their mothers, to excite a freer secretion of milk, or to make it flow.
- Now it is very common with young cats, and not at all rare with old
- cats of the common and Persian breeds (believed by some naturalists to
- be specifically extinct), when comfortably lying on a warm shawl or
- other soft substance, to pound it quietly and alternately with their
- fore-feet; their toes being spread out and claws slightly protruded,
- precisely as when sucking their mother. That it is the same movement is
- clearly shown by their often at the same time taking a bit of the shawl
- into their mouths and sucking it; generally closing their eyes and
- purring from delight. This curious movement is commonly excited only in
- association with the sensation of a warm soft surface; but I have seen
- an old cat, when pleased by having its back scratched, pounding the air
- with its feet in the same manner; so that this action has almost become
- the expression of a pleasurable sensation.
- Having referred to the act of sucking, I may add that this complex
- movement, as well as the alternate protrusion of the fore-feet, are
- reflex actions; for they are performed if a finger moistened with milk
- is placed in the mouth of a puppy, the front part of whose brain has
- been removed.[117] It has recently been stated in France, that the
- action of sucking is excited solely through the sense of smell, so that
- if the olfactory nerves of a puppy are destroyed, it never sucks. In
- like manner the wonderful power which a chicken possesses only a few
- hours after being hatched, of picking up small particles of food, seems
- to be started into action through the sense of hearing; for with
- chickens hatched by artificial heat, a good observer found that “making
- a noise with the finger-nail against a board, in imitation of the
- hen-mother, first taught them to peck at their meat.”[118]
- I will give only one other instance of an habitual and purposeless
- movement. The Sheldrake (_Tadorna_) feeds on the sands left uncovered
- by the tide, and when a worm-cast is discovered, “it begins patting the
- ground with its feet, dancing as it were, over the hole;” and this
- makes the worm come to the surface. Now Mr. St. John says, that when
- his tame Sheldrakes “came to ask for food, they patted the ground in an
- impatient and rapid manner.”[119] This therefore may almost be
- considered as their expression of hunger. Mr. Bartlett informs me that
- the Flamingo and the Kagu (_Rhinochetus jubatus_) when anxious to be
- fed, beat the ground with their feet in the same odd manner. So again
- Kingfishers, when they catch a fish, always beat it until it is killed;
- and in the Zoological Gardens they always beat the raw meat, with which
- they are sometimes fed, before devouring it.
- We have now, I think, sufficiently shown the truth of our first
- Principle, namely, that when any sensation, desire, dislike, &c., has
- led during a long series of generations to some voluntary movement,
- then a tendency to the performance of a similar movement will almost
- certainly be excited, whenever the same, or any analogous or associated
- sensation &c., although very weak, is experienced; notwithstanding that
- the movement in this case may not be of the least use. Such habitual
- movements are often, or generally inherited; and they then differ but
- little from reflex actions. When we treat of the special expressions of
- man, the latter part of our first Principle, as given at the
- commencement of this chapter, will be seen to hold good; namely, that
- when movements, associated through habit with certain states of the
- mind, are partially repressed by the will, the strictly involuntary
- muscles, as well as those which are least under the separate control of
- the will, are liable still to act; and their action is often highly
- expressive. Conversely, when the will is temporarily or permanently
- weakened, the voluntary muscles fail before the involuntary. It is a
- fact familiar to pathologists, as Sir C. Bell remarks,[120] “that when
- debility arises from affection of the brain, the influence is greatest
- on those muscles which are, in their natural condition, most under the
- command of the will.” We shall, also, in our future chapters, consider
- another proposition included in our first Principle; namely, that the
- checking of one habitual movement sometimes requires other slight
- movements; these latter serving as a means of expression.
- CHAPTER II. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION—_continued_.
- The Principle of Antithesis—Instances in the dog and cat—Origin of the
- principle—Conventional signs—The principle of antithesis has not arisen
- from opposite actions being consciously performed under opposite
- impulses.
- We will now consider our second Principle, that of Antithesis. Certain
- states of the mind lead, as we have seen in the last chapter, to
- certain habitual movements which were primarily, or may still be, of
- service; and we shall find that when a directly opposite state of mind
- is induced, there is a strong and involuntary tendency to the
- performance of movements of a directly opposite nature, though these
- have never been of any service. A few striking instances of antithesis
- will be given, when we treat of the special expressions of man; but as,
- in these cases, we are particularly liable to confound conventional or
- artificial gestures and expressions with those which are innate or
- universal, and which alone deserve to rank as true expressions, I will
- in the present chapter almost confine myself to the lower animals.
- Dog in a Hostile Frame of Mind. Fig. 5
- Fig. 6
- Dog in a Hostile Frame of Mind. Fig. 7
- When a dog approaches a strange dog or man in a savage or hostile frame
- of mind be walks upright and very stiffly; his head is slightly raised,
- or not much lowered; the tail is held erect, and quite rigid; the hairs
- bristle, especially along the neck and back; the pricked ears are
- directed forwards, and the eyes have a fixed stare: (see figs. 5 and
- 7). These actions, as will hereafter be explained, follow from the
- dog’s intention to attack his enemy, and are thus to a large extent
- intelligible. As he prepares to spring with a savage growl on his
- enemy, the canine teeth are uncovered, and the ears are pressed close
- backwards on the head; but with these latter actions, we are not here
- concerned. Let us now suppose that the dog suddenly discovers that the
- man he is approaching, is not a stranger, but his master; and let it be
- observed how completely and instantaneously his whole bearing is
- reversed. Instead of walking upright, the body sinks downwards or even
- crouches, and is thrown into flexuous movements; his tail, instead of
- being held stiff and upright, is lowered and wagged from side to side;
- his hair instantly becomes smooth; his ears are depressed and drawn
- backwards, but not closely to the head; and his lips hang loosely. From
- the drawing back of the ears, the eyelids become elongated, and the
- eyes no longer appear round and staring. It should be added that the
- animal is at such times in an excited condition from joy; and
- nerve-force will be generated in excess, which naturally leads to
- action of some kind. Not one of the above movements, so clearly
- expressive of affection, are of the least direct service to the animal.
- They are explicable, as far as I can see, solely from being in complete
- opposition or antithesis to the attitude and movements which, from
- intelligible causes, are assumed when a dog intends to fight, and which
- consequently are expressive of anger. I request the reader to look at
- the four accompanying sketches, which have been given in order to
- recall vividly the appearance of a dog under these two states of mind.
- It is, however, not a little difficult to represent affection in a dog,
- whilst caressing his master and wagging his tail, as the essence of the
- expression lies in the continuous flexuous movements.
- Dog Carressing his Master. Fig. 8
- We will now turn to the cat. When this animal is threatened by a dog,
- it arches its back in a surprising manner, erects its hair, opens its
- mouth and spits. But we are not here concerned with this well-known
- attitude, expressive of terror combined with anger; we are concerned
- only with that of rage or anger. This is not often seen, but may be
- observed when two cats are fighting together; and I have seen it well
- exhibited by a savage cat whilst plagued by a boy. The attitude is
- almost exactly the same as that of a tiger disturbed and growling over
- its food, which every one must have beheld in menageries. The animal
- assumes a crouching position, with the body extended; and the whole
- tail, or the tip alone, is lashed or curled from side to side. The hair
- is not in the least erect. Thus far, the attitude and movements are
- nearly the same as when the animal is prepared to spring on its prey,
- and when, no doubt, it feels savage. But when preparing to fight, there
- is this difference, that the ears are closely pressed backwards; the
- mouth is partially opened, showing the teeth; the fore feet are
- occasionally struck out with protruded claws; and the animal
- occasionally utters a fierce growl. (See figs. 9 and 10.) All, or
- almost all these actions naturally follow (as hereafter to be
- explained), from the cat’s manner and intention of attacking its enemy.
- Cat, Savage, and Prepared to Fight. Fig. 9
- Cat in an Affectionate Frame of Mind. Fig. 10
- Let us now look at a cat in a directly opposite frame of mind, whilst
- feeling affectionate and caressing her master; and mark how opposite is
- her attitude in every respect. She now stands upright with her back
- slightly arched, which makes the hair appear rather rough, but it does
- not bristle; her tail, instead of being extended and lashed from side
- to side, is held quite still and perpendicularly upwards; her ears are
- erect and pointed; her mouth is closed; and she rubs against her master
- with a purr instead of a growl. Let it further be observed how widely
- different is the whole bearing of an affectionate cat from that of a
- dog, when with his body crouching and flexuous, his tail lowered and
- wagging, and ears depressed, he caresses his master. This contrast in
- the attitudes and movements of these two carnivorous animals, under the
- same pleased and affectionate frame of mind, can be explained, as it
- appears to me, solely by their movements standing in complete
- antithesis to those which are naturally assumed, when these animals
- feel savage and are prepared either to fight or to seize their prey.
- In these cases of the dog and cat, there is every reason to believe
- that the gestures both of hostility and affection are innate or
- inherited; for they are almost identically the same in the different
- races of the species, and in all the individuals of the same race, both
- young and old.
- I will here give one other instance of antithesis in expression. I
- formerly possessed a large dog, who, like every other dog, was much
- pleased to go out walking. He showed his pleasure by trotting gravely
- before me with high steps, head much raised, moderately erected ears,
- and tail carried aloft but not stiffly. Not far from my house a path
- branches off to the right, leading to the hot-house, which I used often
- to visit for a few moments, to look at my experimental plants. This was
- always a great disappointment to the dog, as he did not know whether I
- should continue my walk; and the instantaneous and complete change of
- expression which came over him as soon as my body swerved in the least
- towards the path (and I sometimes tried this as an experiment) was
- laughable. His look of dejection was known to every member of the
- family, and was called his _hot-house face_. This consisted in the head
- drooping much, the whole body sinking a little and remaining
- motionless; the ears and tail falling suddenly down, but the tail was
- by no means wagged. With the falling of the ears and of his great
- chaps, the eyes became much changed in appearance, and I fancied that
- they looked less bright. His aspect was that of piteous, hopeless
- dejection; and it was, as I have said, laughable, as the cause was so
- slight. Every detail in his attitude was in complete opposition to his
- former joyful yet dignified bearing; and can be explained, as it
- appears to me, in no other way, except through the principle of
- antithesis. Had not the change been so instantaneous, I should have
- attributed it to his lowered spirits affecting, as in the case of man,
- the nervous system and circulation, and consequently the tone of his
- whole muscular frame; and this may have been in part the cause.
- We will now consider how the principle of antithesis in expression has
- arisen. With social animals, the power of intercommunication between
- the members of the same community,—and with other species, between the
- opposite sexes, as well as between the young and the old,—is of the
- highest importance to them. This is generally effected by means of the
- voice, but it is certain that gestures and expressions are to a certain
- extent mutually intelligible. Man not only uses inarticulate cries,
- gestures, and expressions, but has invented articulate language; if,
- indeed, the word INVENTED can be applied to a process, completed by
- innumerable steps, half-consciously made. Any one who has watched
- monkeys will not doubt that they perfectly understand each other’s
- gestures and expression, and to a large extent, as Rengger
- asserts,[201] those of man. An animal when going to attack another, or
- when afraid of another, often makes itself appear terrible, by erecting
- its hair, thus increasing the apparent bulk of its body, by showing its
- teeth, or brandishing its horns, or by uttering fierce sounds.
- As the power of intercommunication is certainly of high service to many
- animals, there is no _à priori_ improbability in the supposition, that
- gestures manifestly of an opposite nature to those by which certain
- feelings are already expressed, should at first have been voluntarily
- employed under the influence of an opposite state of feeling. The fact
- of the gestures being now innate, would be no valid objection to the
- belief that they were at first intentional; for if practised during
- many generations, they would probably at last be inherited.
- Nevertheless it is more than doubtful, as we shall immediately see,
- whether any of the cases which come under our present head of
- antithesis, have thus originated.
- With conventional signs which are not innate, such as those used by the
- deaf and dumb and by savages, the principle of opposition or antithesis
- has been partially brought into play. The Cistercian monks thought it
- sinful to speak, and as they could not avoid holding some
- communication, they invented a gesture language, in which the principle
- of opposition seems to have been employed.[202] Dr. Scott, of the
- Exeter Deaf and Dumb Institution, writes to me that “opposites are
- greatly used in teaching the deaf and dumb, who have a lively sense of
- them.” Nevertheless I have been surprised how few unequivocal instances
- can be adduced. This depends partly on all the signs having commonly
- had some natural origin; and partly on the practice of the deaf and
- dumb and of savages to contract their signs as much as possible for the
- sake of rapidity.[203] Hence their natural source or origin often
- becomes doubtful or is completely lost; as is likewise the case with
- articulate language.
- Many signs, moreover, which plainly stand in opposition to each other,
- appear to have had on both sides a significant origin. This seems to
- hold good with the signs used by the deal and dumb for light and
- darkness, for strength and weakness, &c. In a future chapter I shall
- endeavour to show that the opposite gestures of affirmation and
- negation, namely, vertically nodding and laterally shaking the head,
- have both probably had a natural beginning. The waving of the hand from
- right to left, which is used as a negative by some savages, may have
- been invented in imitation of shaking the head; but whether the
- opposite movement of waving the hand in a straight line from the face,
- which is used in affirmation, has arisen through antithesis or in some
- quite distinct manner, is doubtful.
- If we now turn to the gestures which are innate or common to all the
- individuals of the same species, and which come under the present head
- of antithesis, it is extremely doubtful, whether any of them were at
- first deliberately invented and consciously performed. With mankind the
- best instance of a gesture standing in direct opposition to other
- movements, naturally assumed under an opposite frame of mind, is that
- of shrugging the shoulders. This expresses impotence or an
- apology,—something which cannot be done, or cannot be avoided. The
- gesture is sometimes used consciously and voluntarily, but it is
- extremely improbable that it was at first deliberately invented, and
- afterwards fixed by habit; for not only do young children sometimes
- shrug their shoulders under the above states of mind, but the movement
- is accompanied, as will be shown in a future chapter, by various
- subordinate movements, which not one man in a thousand is aware of,
- unless he has specially attended to the subject.
- Dogs when approaching a strange dog, may find it useful to show by
- their movements that they are friendly, and do not wish to fight. When
- two young dogs in play are growling and biting each other’s faces and
- legs, it is obvious that they mutually understand each other’s gestures
- and manners. There seems, indeed, some degree of instinctive knowledge
- in puppies and kittens, that they must not use their sharp little teeth
- or claws too freely in their play, though this sometimes happens and a
- squeal is the result; otherwise they would often injure each other’s
- eyes. When my terrier bites my hand in play, often snarling at the same
- time, if he bites too hard and I say GENTLY, GENTLY, he goes on biting,
- but answers me by a few wags of the tail, which seems to say “Never
- mind, it is all fun.” Although dogs do thus express, and may wish to
- express, to other dogs and to man, that they are in a friendly state of
- mind, it is incredible that they could ever have deliberately thought
- of drawing back and depressing their ears, instead of holding them
- erect,—of lowering and wagging their tails, instead of keeping them
- stiff and upright, &c., because they knew that these movements stood in
- direct opposition to those assumed under an opposite and savage frame
- of mind.
- Again, when a cat, or rather when some early progenitor of the species,
- from feeling affectionate first slightly arched its back, held its tail
- perpendicularly upwards and pricked its ears, can it be believed that
- the animal consciously wished thus to show that its frame of mind was
- directly the reverse of that, when from being ready to fight or to
- spring on its prey, it assumed a crouching attitude, curled its tail
- from side to side and depressed its ears? Even still less can I believe
- that my dog voluntarily put on his dejected attitude and “_hot-house
- face_,” which formed so complete a contrast to his previous cheerful
- attitude and whole bearing. It cannot be supposed that he knew that I
- should understand his expression, and that he could thus soften my
- heart and make me give up visiting the hot-house.
- Hence for the development of the movements which come under the present
- head, some other principle, distinct from the will and consciousness,
- must have intervened. This principle appears to be that every movement
- which we have voluntarily performed throughout our lives has required
- the action of certain muscles; and when we have performed a directly
- opposite movement, an opposite set of muscles has been habitually
- brought into play,—as in turning to the right or to the left, in
- pushing away or pulling an object towards us, and in lifting or
- lowering a weight. So strongly are our intentions and movements
- associated together, that if we eagerly wish an object to move in any
- direction, we can hardly avoid moving our bodies in the same direction,
- although we may be perfectly aware that this can have no influence. A
- good illustration of this fact has already been given in the
- Introduction, namely, in the grotesque movements of a young and eager
- billiard-player, whilst watching the course of his ball. A man or child
- in a passion, if he tells any one in a loud voice to begone, generally
- moves his arm as if to push him away, although the offender may not be
- standing near, and although there may be not the least need to explain
- by a gesture what is meant. On the other hand, if we eagerly desire
- some one to approach us closely, we act as if pulling him towards us;
- and so in innumerable other instances.
- As the performance of ordinary movements of an opposite kind, under
- opposite impulses of the will, has become habitual in us and in the
- lower animals, so when actions of one kind have become firmly
- associated with any sensation or emotion, it appears natural that
- actions of a directly opposite kind, though of no use, should be
- unconsciously performed through habit and association, under the
- influence of a directly opposite sensation or emotion. On this
- principle alone can I understand how the gestures and expressions which
- come under the present head of antithesis have originated. If indeed
- they are serviceable to man or to any other animal, in aid of
- inarticulate cries or language, they will likewise be voluntarily
- employed, and the habit will thus be strengthened. But whether or not
- of service as a means of communication, the tendency to perform
- opposite movements under opposite sensations or emotions would, if we
- may judge by analogy, become hereditary through long practice; and
- there cannot be a doubt that several expressive movements due to the
- principle of antithesis are inherited.
- CHAPTER III. GENERAL PRINCIPLES OF EXPRESSION—_concluded_.
- The principle of direct action of the excited nervous system on the
- body, independently of the will and in part of habit—Change of colour
- in the hair—Trembling of the muscles—Modified
- secretions—Perspiration—Expression of extreme pain—Of rage, great joy,
- and terror—Contrast between the emotions which cause and do not cause
- expressive movements—Exciting and depressing states of the
- mind—Summary.
- We now come to our third Principle, namely, that certain actions which
- we recognize as expressive of certain states of the mind, are the
- direct result of the constitution of the nervous system, and have been
- from the first independent of the will, and, to a large extent, of
- habit. When the sensorium is strongly excited nerve-force is generated
- in excess, and is transmitted in certain directions, dependent on the
- connection of the nerve-cells, and, as far as the muscular system is
- concerned, on the nature of the movements which have been habitually
- practised. Or the supply of nerve-force may, as it appears, be
- interrupted. Of course every movement which we make is determined by
- the constitution of the nervous system; but actions performed in
- obedience to the will, or through habit, or through the principle of
- antithesis, are here as far as possible excluded. Our present subject
- is very obscure, but, from its importance, must be discussed at some
- little length; and it is always advisable to perceive clearly our
- ignorance.
- The most striking case, though a rare and abnormal one, which can be
- adduced of the direct influence of the nervous system, when strongly
- affected, on the body, is the loss of colour in the hair, which has
- occasionally been observed after extreme terror or grief. One authentic
- instance has been recorded, in the case of a man brought out for
- execution in India, in which the change of colour was so rapid that it
- was perceptible to the eye.[301]
- Another good case is that of the trembling of the muscles, which is
- common to man and to many, or most, of the lower animals. Trembling is
- of no service, often of much disservice, and cannot have been at first
- acquired through the will, and then rendered habitual in association
- with any emotion. I am assured by an eminent authority that young
- children do not tremble, but go into convulsions under the
- circumstances which would induce excessive trembling in adults.
- Trembling is excited in different individuals in very different degrees
- and by the most diversified causes,—by cold to the surface, before
- fever-fits, although the temperature of the body is then above the
- normal standard; in blood-poisoning, delirium tremens, and other
- diseases; by general failure of power in old age; by exhaustion after
- excessive fatigue; locally from severe injuries, such as burns; and, in
- an especial manner, by the passage of a catheter. Of all emotions, fear
- notoriously is the most apt to induce trembling; but so do occasionally
- great anger and joy. I remember once seeing a boy who had just shot his
- first snipe on the wing, and his hands trembled to such a degree from
- delight, that he could not for some time reload his gun; and I have
- heard of an exactly similar case with an Australian savage, to whom a
- gun had been lent. Fine music, from the vague emotions thus excited,
- causes a shiver to run down the backs of some persons. There seems to
- be very little in common in the above several physical causes and
- emotions to account for trembling; and Sir J. Paget, to whom I am
- indebted for several of the above statements, informs me that the
- subject is a very obscure one. As trembling is sometimes caused by
- rage, long before exhaustion can have set in, and as it sometimes
- accompanies great joy, it would appear that any strong excitement of
- the nervous system interrupts the steady flow of nerve-force to the
- muscles.[302]
- The manner in which the secretions of the alimentary canal and of
- certain glands—as the liver, kidneys, or mammæ are affected by strong
- emotions, is another excellent instance of the direct action of the
- sensorium on these organs, independently of the will or of any
- serviceable associated habit. There is the greatest difference in
- different persons in the parts which are thus affected, and in the
- degree of their affection.
- The heart, which goes on uninterruptedly beating night and day in so
- wonderful a manner, is extremely sensitive to external stimulants. The
- great physiologist, Claude Bernard,[303] has shown how the least
- excitement of a sensitive nerve reacts on the heart; even when a nerve
- is touched so slightly that no pain can possibly be felt by the animal
- under experiment. Hence when the mind is strongly excited, we might
- expect that it would instantly affect in a direct manner the heart; and
- this is universally acknowledged and felt to be the case. Claude
- Bernard also repeatedly insists, and this deserves especial notice,
- that when the heart is affected it reacts on the brain; and the state
- of the brain again reacts through the pneumo-gastric nerve on the
- heart; so that under any excitement there will be much mutual action
- and reaction between these, the two most important organs of the body.
- The vaso-motor system, which regulates the diameter of the small
- arteries, is directly acted on by the sensorium, as we see when a man
- blushes from shame; but in this latter case the checked transmission of
- nerve-force to the vessels of the face can, I think, be partly
- explained in a curious manner through habit. We shall also be able to
- throw some light, though very little, on the involuntary erection of
- the hair under the emotions of terror and rage. The secretion of tears
- depends, no doubt, on the connection of certain nerve-cells; but here
- again we can trace some few of the steps by which the flow of
- nerve-force through the requisite channels has become habitual under
- certain emotions.
- A brief consideration of the outward signs of some of the stronger
- sensations and emotions will best serve to show us, although vaguely,
- in how complex a manner the principle under consideration of the direct
- action of the excited nervous system of the body, is combined with the
- principle of habitually associated, serviceable movements.
- When animals suffer from an agony of pain, they generally writhe about
- with frightful contortions; and those which habitually use their voices
- utter piercing cries or groans. Almost every muscle of the body is
- brought into strong action. With man the mouth may be closely
- compressed, or more commonly the lips are retracted, with the teeth
- clenched or ground together. There is said to be “gnashing of teeth” in
- hell; and I have plainly heard the grinding of the molar teeth of a cow
- which was suffering acutely from inflammation of the bowels. The female
- hippopotamus in the Zoological Gardens, when she produced her young,
- suffered greatly; she incessantly walked about, or rolled on her sides,
- opening and closing her jaws, and clattering her teeth together.[304]
- With man the eyes stare wildly as in horrified astonishment, or the
- brows are heavily contracted. Perspiration bathes the body, and drops
- trickle down the face. The circulation and respiration are much
- affected. Hence the nostrils are generally dilated and often quiver; or
- the breath may be held until the blood stagnates in the purple face. If
- the agony be severe and prolonged, these signs all change; utter
- prostration follows, with fainting or convulsions.
- A sensitive nerve when irritated transmits some influence to the
- nerve-cell, whence it proceeds; and this transmits its influence, first
- to the corresponding nerve-cell on the opposite side of the body, and
- then upwards and downwards along the cerebro-spinal column to other
- nerve-cells, to a greater or less extent, according to the strength of
- the excitement; so that, ultimately, the whole nervous system maybe
- affected.[305] This involuntary transmission of nerve-force may or may
- not be accompanied by consciousness. Why the irritation of a nerve-cell
- should generate or liberate nerve-force is not known; but that this is
- the case seems to be the conclusion arrived at by all the greatest
- physiologists, such as Müller, Virchow, Bernard, &c.[306] As Mr.
- Herbert Spencer remarks, it may be received as an “unquestionable truth
- that, at any moment, the existing quantity of liberated nerve-force,
- which in an inscrutable way produces in us the state we call feeling,
- MUST expend itself in some direction—MUST generate an equivalent
- manifestation of force somewhere;” so that, when the cerebro-spinal
- system is highly excited and nerve-force is liberated in excess, it may
- be expended in intense sensations, active thought, violent movements,
- or increased activity of the glands.[307] Mr. Spencer further maintains
- that an “overflow of nerve-force, undirected by any motive, will
- manifestly take the most habitual routes; and, if these do not suffice,
- will next overflow into the less habitual ones.” Consequently the
- facial and respiratory muscles, which are the most used, will be apt to
- be first brought into action; then those of the upper extremities, next
- those of the lower, and finally those of the whole body.[308]
- An emotion may be very strong, but it will have little tendency to
- induce movements of any kind, if it has not commonly led to voluntary
- action for its relief or gratification; and when movements are excited,
- their nature is, to a large extent, determined by those which have
- often and voluntarily been performed for some definite end under the
- same emotion. Great pain urges all animals, and has urged them during
- endless generations, to make the most violent and diversified efforts
- to escape from the cause of suffering. Even when a limb or other
- separate part of the body is hurt, we often see a tendency to shake it,
- as if to shake off the cause, though this may obviously be impossible.
- Thus a habit of exerting with the utmost force all the muscles will
- have been established, whenever great suffering is experienced. As the
- muscles of the chest and vocal organs are habitually used, these will
- be particularly liable to be acted on, and loud, harsh screams or cries
- will be uttered. But the advantage derived from outcries has here
- probably come into play in an important manner; for the young of most
- animals, when in distress or danger, call loudly to their parents for
- aid, as do the members of the same community for mutual aid.
- Another principle, namely, the internal consciousness that the power or
- capacity of the nervous system is limited, will have strengthened,
- though in a subordinate degree, the tendency to violent action under
- extreme suffering. A man cannot think deeply and exert his utmost
- muscular force. As Hippocrates long ago observed, if two pains are felt
- at the same time, the severer one dulls the other. Martyrs, in the
- ecstasy of their religious fervour have often, as it would appear, been
- insensible to the most horrid tortures. Sailors who are going to be
- flogged sometimes take a piece of lead into their mouths, in order to
- bite it with their utmost force, and thus to bear the pain. Parturient
- women prepare to exert their muscles to the utmost in order to relieve
- their sufferings.
- We thus see that the undirected radiation of nerve-force from the
- nerve-cells which are first affected—the long-continued habit of
- attempting by struggling to escape from the cause of suffering—and the
- consciousness that voluntary muscular exertion relieves pain, have all
- probably concurred in giving a tendency to the most violent, almost
- convulsive, movements under extreme suffering; and such movements,
- including those of the vocal organs, are universally recognized as
- highly expressive of this condition.
- As the mere touching of a sensitive nerve reacts in a direct manner on
- the heart, severe pain will obviously react on it in like manner, but
- far more energetically. Nevertheless, even in this case, we must not
- overlook the indirect effects of habit on the heart, as we shall see
- when we consider the signs of rage.
- When a man suffers from an agony of pain, the perspiration often
- trickles down his face; and I have been assured by a veterinary surgeon
- that he has frequently seen drops falling from the belly and running
- down the inside of the thighs of horses, and from the bodies of cattle,
- when thus suffering. He has observed this, when there has been no
- struggling which would account for the perspiration. The whole body of
- the female hippopotamus, before alluded to, was covered with
- red-coloured perspiration whilst giving birth to her young. So it is
- with extreme fear; the same veterinary has often seen horses sweating
- from this cause; as has Mr. Bartlett with the rhinoceros; and with man
- it is a well-known symptom. The cause of perspiration bursting forth in
- these cases is quite obscure; but it is thought by some physiologists
- to be connected with the failing power of the capillary circulation;
- and we know that the vasomotor system, which regulates the capillary
- circulation, is much influenced by the mind. With respect to the
- movements of certain muscles of the face under great suffering, as well
- as from other emotions, these will be best considered when we treat of
- the special expressions of man and of the lower animals.
- We will now turn to the characteristic symptoms of Rage. Under this
- powerful emotion the action of the heart is much accelerated,[309] or
- it may be much disturbed. The face reddens, or it becomes purple from
- the impeded return of the blood, or may turn deadly pale. The
- respiration is laboured, the chest heaves, and the dilated nostrils
- quiver. The whole body often trembles. The voice is affected. The teeth
- are clenched or ground together, and the muscular system is commonly
- stimulated to violent, almost frantic action. But the gestures of a man
- in this state usually differ from the purposeless writhings and
- struggles of one suffering from an agony of pain; for they represent
- more or less plainly the act of striking or fighting with an enemy.
- All these signs of rage are probably in large part, and some of them
- appear to be wholly, due to the direct action of the excited sensorium.
- But animals of all kinds, and their progenitors before them, when
- attacked or threatened by an enemy, have exerted their utmost powers in
- fighting and in defending themselves. Unless an animal does thus act,
- or has the intention, or at least the desire, to attack its enemy, it
- cannot properly be said to be enraged. An inherited habit of muscular
- exertion will thus have been gained in association with rage; and this
- will directly or indirectly affect various organs, in nearly the same
- manner as does great bodily suffering.
- The heart no doubt will likewise be affected in a direct manner; but it
- will also in all probability be affected through habit; and all the
- more so from not being under the control of the will. We know that any
- great exertion which we voluntarily make, affects the heart, through
- mechanical and other principles which need not here be considered; and
- it was shown in the first chapter that nerve-force flows readily
- through habitually used channels,—through the nerves of voluntary or
- involuntary movement, and through those of sensation. Thus even a
- moderate amount of exertion will tend to act on the heart; and on the
- principle of association, of which so many instances have been given,
- we may feel nearly sure that any sensation or emotion, as great pain or
- rage, which has habitually led to much muscular action, will
- immediately influence the flow of nerve-force to the heart, although
- there may not be at the time any muscular exertion.
- The heart, as I have said, will be all the more readily affected
- through habitual associations, as it is not under the control of the
- will. A man when moderately angry, or even when enraged, may command
- the movements of his body, but he cannot prevent his heart from beating
- rapidly. His chest will perhaps give a few heaves, and his nostrils
- just quiver, for the movements of respiration are only in part
- voluntary. In like manner those muscles of the face which are least
- obedient to the will, will sometimes alone betray a slight and passing
- emotion. The glands again are wholly independent of the will, and a man
- suffering from grief may command his features, but cannot always
- prevent the tears from coming into his eyes. A hungry man, if tempting
- food is placed before him, may not show his hunger by any outward
- gesture, but he cannot check the secretion of saliva.
- Under a transport of Joy or of vivid Pleasure, there is a strong
- tendency to various purposeless movements, and to the utterance of
- various sounds. We see this in our young children, in their loud
- laughter, clapping of hands, and jumping for joy; in the bounding and
- barking of a dog when going out to walk with his master; and in the
- frisking of a horse when turned out into an open field. Joy quickens
- the circulation, and this stimulates the brain, which again reacts on
- the whole body. The above purposeless movements and increased
- heart-action may be attributed in chief part to the excited state of
- the sensorium,[310] and to the consequent undirected overflow, as Mr.
- Herbert Spencer insists, of nerve-force. It deserves notice, that it is
- chiefly the anticipation of a pleasure, and not its actual enjoyment,
- which leads to purposeless and extravagant movements of the body, and
- to the utterance of various sounds. We see this in our children when
- they expect any great pleasure or treat; and dogs, which have been
- bounding about at the sight of a plate of food, when they get it do not
- show their delight by any outward sign, not even by wagging their
- tails. Now with animals of all kinds, the acquirement of almost all
- their pleasures, with the exception of those of warmth and rest, are
- associated, and have long been associated with active movements, as in
- the hunting or search for food, and in their courtship. Moreover, the
- mere exertion of the muscles after long rest or confinement is in
- itself a pleasure, as we ourselves feel, and as we see in the play of
- young animals. Therefore on this latter principle alone we might
- perhaps expect, that vivid pleasure would be apt to show itself
- conversely in muscular movements.
- With all or almost all animals, even with birds, Terror causes the body
- to tremble. The skin becomes pale, sweat breaks out, and the hair
- bristles. The secretions of the alimentary canal and of the kidneys are
- increased, and they are involuntarily voided, owing to the relaxation
- of the sphincter muscles, as is known to be the case with man, and as I
- have seen with cattle, dogs, cats, and monkeys. The breathing is
- hurried. The heart beats quickly, wildly, and violently; but whether it
- pumps the blood more efficiently through the body may be doubted, for
- the surface seems bloodless and the strength of the muscles soon fails.
- In a frightened horse I have felt through the saddle the beating of the
- heart so plainly that I could have counted the beats. The mental
- faculties are much disturbed. Utter prostration soon follows, and even
- fainting. A terrified canary-bird has been seen not only to tremble and
- to turn white about the base of the bill, but to faint;[311] and I once
- caught a robin in a room, which fainted so completely, that for a time
- I thought it dead.
- Most of these symptoms are probably the direct result, independently of
- habit, of the disturbed state of the sensorium; but it is doubtful
- whether they ought to be wholly thus accounted for. When an animal is
- alarmed it almost always stands motionless for a moment, in order to
- collect its senses and to ascertain the source of danger, and sometimes
- for the sake of escaping detection. But headlong flight soon follows,
- with no husbanding of the strength as in fighting, and the animal
- continues to fly as long as the danger lasts, until utter prostration,
- with failing respiration and circulation, with all the muscles
- quivering and profuse sweating, renders further flight impossible.
- Hence it does not seem improbable that the principle of associated
- habit may in part account for, or at least augment, some of the
- above-named characteristic symptoms of extreme terror.
- That the principle of associated habit has played an important part in
- causing the movements expressive of the foregoing several strong
- emotions and sensations, we may, I think, conclude from considering
- firstly, some other strong emotions which do not ordinarily require for
- their relief or gratification any voluntary movement; and secondly the
- contrast in nature between the so-called exciting and depressing states
- of the mind. No emotion is stronger than maternal love; but a mother
- may feel the deepest love for her helpless infant, and yet not show it
- by any outward sign; or only by slight caressing movements, with a
- gentle smile and tender eyes. But let any one intentionally injure her
- infant, and see what a change! how she starts up with threatening
- aspect, how her eyes sparkle and her face reddens, how her bosom
- heaves, nostrils dilate, and heart beats; for anger, and not maternal
- love, has habitually led to action. The love between the opposite sexes
- is widely different from maternal love; and when lovers meet, we know
- that their hearts beat quickly, their breathing is hurried, and their
- faces flush; for this love is not inactive like that of a mother for
- her infant.
- A man may have his mind filled with the blackest hatred or suspicion,
- or be corroded with envy or jealousy, but as these feelings do not at
- once lead to action, and as they commonly last for some time, they are
- not shown by any outward sign, excepting that a man in this state
- assuredly does not appear cheerful or good-tempered. If indeed these
- feelings break out into overt acts, rage takes their place, and will be
- plainly exhibited. Painters can hardly portray suspicion, jealousy,
- envy, &c., except by the aid of accessories which tell the tale; and
- poets use such vague and fanciful expressions as “green-eyed jealousy.”
- Spenser describes suspicion as “Foul, ill-favoured, and grim, under his
- eyebrows looking still askance,” &c.; Shakespeare speaks of envy “as
- lean-faced in her loathsome case;” and in another place he says, “no
- black envy shall make my grave;” and again as “above pale envy’s
- threatening reach.”
- Emotions and sensations have often been classed as exciting or
- depressing. When all the organs of the body and mind,—those of
- voluntary and involuntary movement, of perception, sensation, thought,
- &c.,—perform their functions more energetically and rapidly than usual,
- a man or animal may be said to be excited, and, under an opposite
- state, to be depressed. Anger and joy are from the first exciting
- emotions, and they naturally lead, more especially the former, to
- energetic movements, which react on the heart and this again on the
- brain. A physician once remarked to me as a proof of the exciting
- nature of anger, that a man when excessively jaded will sometimes
- invent imaginary offences and put himself into a passion, unconsciously
- for the sake of reinvigorating himself; and since hearing this remark,
- I have occasionally recognized its full truth.
- Several other states of mind appear to be at first exciting, but soon
- become depressing to an extreme degree. When a mother suddenly loses
- her child, sometimes she is frantic with grief, and must be considered
- to be in an excited state; she walks wildly about, tears her hair or
- clothes, and wrings her hands. This latter action is perhaps due to the
- principle of antithesis, betraying an inward sense of helplessness and
- that nothing can be done. The other wild and violent movements may be
- in part explained by the relief experienced through muscular exertion,
- and in part by the undirected overflow of nerve-force from the excited
- sensorium. But under the sudden loss of a beloved person, one of the
- first and commonest thoughts which occurs, is that something more might
- have been done to save the lost one. An excellent observer,[312] in
- describing the behaviour of a girl at the sudden death of her father,
- says she “went about the house wringing her hands like a creature
- demented, saying ‘It was her fault;’ ‘I should never have left him;’
- ‘If I had only sat up with him,’” &c. With such ideas vividly present
- before the mind, there would arise, through the principle of associated
- habit, the strongest tendency to energetic action of some kind.
- As soon as the sufferer is fully conscious that nothing can be done,
- despair or deep sorrow takes the place of frantic grief. The sufferer
- sits motionless, or gently rocks to and fro; the circulation becomes
- languid; respiration is almost forgotten, and deep sighs are drawn. All
- this reacts on the brain, and prostration soon follows with collapsed
- muscles and dulled eyes. As associated habit no longer prompts the
- sufferer to action, he is urged by his friends to voluntary exertion,
- and not to give way to silent, motionless grief. Exertion stimulates
- the hear, and this reacts on the brain, and aids the mind to bear its
- heavy load.
- Pain, if severe, soon induces extreme depression or prostration; but it
- is at first a stimulant and excites to action, as we see when we whip a
- horse, and as is shown by the horrid tortures inflicted in foreign
- lands on exhausted dray-bullocks, to rouse them to renewed exertion.
- Fear again is the most depressing of all the emotions; and it soon
- induces utter, helpless prostration, as if in consequence of, or in
- association with, the most violent and prolonged attempts to escape
- from the danger, though no such attempts have actually been made.
- Nevertheless, even extreme fear often acts at first as a powerful
- stimulant. A man or animal driven through terror to desperation, is
- endowed with wonderful strength, and is notoriously dangerous in the
- highest degree.
- On the whole we may conclude that the principle of the direct action of
- the sensorium on the body, due to the constitution of the nervous
- system, and from the first independent of the will, has been highly
- influential in determining many expressions. Good instances are
- afforded by the trembling of the muscles, the sweating of the skin, the
- modified secretions of the alimentary canal and glands, under various
- emotions and sensations. But actions of this kind are often combined
- with others, which follow from our first principle, namely, that
- actions which have often been of direct or indirect service, under
- certain states of the mind, in order to gratify or relieve certain
- sensations, desires, &c., are still performed under analogous
- circumstances through mere habit although of no service. We have
- combinations of this kind, at least in part, in the frantic gestures of
- rage and in the writhings of extreme pain; and, perhaps, in the
- increased action of the heart and of the respiratory organs. Even when
- these and other emotions or sensations are aroused in a very feeble
- manner, there will still be a tendency to similar actions, owing to the
- force of long-associated habit; and those actions which are least under
- voluntary control will generally be longest retained. Our second
- principle of antithesis has likewise occasionally come into play.
- Finally, so many expressive movements can be explained, as I trust will
- be seen in the course of this volume, through the three principles
- which have now been discussed, that we may hope hereafter to see all
- thus explained, or by closely analogous principles. It is, however,
- often impossible to decide how much weight ought to be attributed, in
- each particular case, to one of our principles, and how much to
- another; and very many points in the theory of Expression remain
- inexplicable.
- CHAPTER IV. MEANS OF EXPRESSION IN ANIMALS.
- The emission of Sounds—Vocal sounds—Sounds otherwise produced—Erection
- of the dermal appendages, hairs, feathers, &c., under the emotions of
- anger and terror—The drawing back of the ears as a preparation for
- fighting, and as an expression of anger—Erection of the ears and
- raising the head, a sign of attention.
- In this and the following chapter I will describe, but only in
- sufficient detail to illustrate my subject, the expressive movements,
- under different states of the mind, of some few well-known animals. But
- before considering them in due succession, it will save much useless
- repetition to discuss certain means of expression common to most of
- them.
- _The emission of Sounds_.—With many kinds of animals, man included, the
- vocal organs are efficient in the highest degree as a means of
- expression. We have seen, in the last chapter, that when the sensorium
- is strongly excited, the muscles of the body are generally thrown into
- violent action; and as a consequence, loud sounds are uttered, however
- silent the animal may generally be, and although the sounds may be of
- no use. Hares and rabbits for instance, never, I believe, use their
- vocal organs except in the extremity of suffering; as, when a wounded
- hare is killed by the sportsman, or when a young rabbit is caught by a
- stoat. Cattle and horses suffer great pain in silence; but when this is
- excessive, and especially when associated with terror, they utter
- fearful sounds. I have often recognized, from a distance on the Pampas,
- the agonized death-bellow of the cattle, when caught by the lasso and
- hamstrung. It is said that horses, when attacked by wolves, utter loud
- and peculiar screams of distress.
- Involuntary and purposeless contractions of the muscles of the chest
- and glottis, excited in the above manner, may have first given rise to
- the emission of vocal sounds. But the voice is now largely used by many
- animals for various purposes; and habit seems to have played an
- important part in its employment under other circumstances. Naturalists
- have remarked, I believe with truth, that social animals, from
- habitually using their vocal organs as a means of intercommunication,
- use them on other occasions much more freely than other animals. But
- there are marked exceptions to this rule, for instance, with the
- rabbit. The principle, also, of association, which is so widely
- extended in its power, has likewise played its part. Hence it follows
- that the voice, from having been habitually employed as a serviceable
- aid under certain conditions, inducing pleasure, pain, rage, &c., is
- commonly used whenever the same sensations or emotions are excited,
- under quite different conditions, or in a lesser degree.
- The sexes of many animals incessantly call for each other during the
- breeding-season; and in not a few cases, the male endeavours thus to
- charm or excite the female. This, indeed, seems to have been the
- primeval use and means of development of the voice, as I have attempted
- to show in my ‘Descent of Man.’ Thus the use of the vocal organs will
- have become associated with the anticipation of the strongest pleasure
- which animals are capable of feeling. Animals which live in society
- often call to each other when separated, and evidently feel much joy at
- meeting; as we see with a horse, on the return of his companion, for
- whom he has been neighing. The mother calls incessantly for her lost
- young ones; for instance, a cow for her calf; and the young of many
- animals call for their mothers. When a flock of sheep is scattered, the
- ewes bleat incessantly for their lambs, and their mutual pleasure at
- coming together is manifest. Woe betide the man who meddles with the
- young of the larger and fiercer quadrupeds, if they hear the cry of
- distress from their young. Rage leads to the violent exertion of all
- the muscles, including those of the voice; and some animals, when
- enraged, endeavour to strike terror into their enemies by its power and
- harshness, as the lion does by roaring, and the dog by growling. I
- infer that their object is to strike terror, because the lion at the
- same time erects the hair of its mane, and the dog the hair along its
- back, and thus they make themselves appear as large and terrible as
- possible. Rival males try to excel and challenge each other by their
- voices, and this leads to deadly contests. Thus the use of the voice
- will have become associated with the emotion of anger, however it may
- be aroused. We have also seen that intense pain, like rage, leads to
- violent outcries, and the exertion of screaming by itself gives some
- relief; and thus the use of the voice will have become associated with
- suffering of any kind.
- The cause of widely different sounds being uttered under different
- emotions and sensations is a very obscure subject. Nor does the rule
- always hold good that there is any marked difference. For instance with
- the dog, the bark of anger and that of joy do not differ much, though
- they can be distinguished. It is not probable that any precise
- explanation of the cause or source of each particular sound, under
- different states of the mind, will ever be given. We know that some
- animals, after being domesticated, have acquired the habit of uttering
- sounds which were not natural to them.[401] Thus domestic dogs, and
- even tamed jackals, have learnt to bark, which is a noise not proper to
- any species of the genus, with the exception of the _Canis latrans_ of
- North America, which is said to bark. Some breeds, also, of the
- domestic pigeon have learnt to coo in a new and quite peculiar manner.
- The character of the human voice, under the influence of various
- emotions, has been discussed by Mr. Herbert Spencer[402] in his
- interesting essay on Music. He clearly shows that the voice alters much
- under different conditions, in loudness and in quality, that is, in
- resonance and _timbre_, in pitch and intervals. No one can listen to an
- eloquent orator or preacher, or to a man calling angrily to another, or
- to one expressing astonishment, without being struck with the truth of
- Mr. Spencer’s remarks. It is curious how early in life the modulation
- of the voice becomes expressive. With one of my children, under the age
- of two years, I clearly perceived that his humph of assent was rendered
- by a slight modulation strongly emphatic; and that by a peculiar whine
- his negative expressed obstinate determination. Mr. Spencer further
- shows that emotional speech, in all the above respects is intimately
- related to vocal music, and consequently to instrumental music; and he
- attempts to explain the characteristic qualities of both on
- physiological grounds—namely, on “the general law that a feeling is a
- stimulus to muscular action.” It may be admitted that the voice is
- affected through this law; but the explanation appears to me too
- general and vague to throw much light on the various differences, with
- the exception of that of loudness, between ordinary speech and
- emotional speech, or singing.
- This remark holds good, whether we believe that the various qualities
- of the voice originated in speaking under the excitement of strong
- feelings, and that these qualities have subsequently been transferred
- to vocal music; or whether we believe, as I maintain, that the habit of
- uttering musical sounds was first developed, as a means of courtship,
- in the early progenitors of man, and thus became associated with the
- strongest emotions of which they were capable,—namely, ardent love,
- rivalry and triumph. That animals utter musical notes is familiar to
- every one, as we may daily hear in the singing of birds. It is a more
- remarkable fact that an ape, one of the Gibbons, produces an exact
- octave of musical sounds, ascending and descending the scale by
- halftones; so that this monkey “alone of brute mammals may be said to
- sing.”[403] From this fact, and from the analogy of other animals, I
- have been led to infer that the progenitors of man probably uttered
- musical tones, before they had acquired the power of articulate speech;
- and that consequently, when the voice is used under any strong emotion,
- it tends to assume, through the principle of association, a musical
- character. We can plainly perceive, with some of the lower animals,
- that the males employ their voices to please the females, and that they
- themselves take pleasure in their own vocal utterances; but why
- particular sounds are uttered, and why these give pleasure cannot at
- present be explained.
- That the pitch of the voice bears some relation to certain states of
- feeling is tolerably clear. A person gently complaining of
- ill-treatment, or slightly suffering, almost always speaks in a
- high-pitched voice. Dogs, when a little impatient, often make a high
- piping note through their noses, which at once strikes us as
- plaintive;[404] but how difficult it is to know whether the sound is
- essentially plaintive, or only appears so in this particular case, from
- our having learnt by experience what it means! Rengger, states[405]
- that the monkeys (_Cebus azaræ_), which he kept in Paraguay, expressed
- astonishment by a half-piping, half-snarling noise; anger or
- impatience, by repeating the sound _hu hu_ in a deeper, grunting voice;
- and fright or pain, by shrill screams. On the other hand, with mankind,
- deep groans and high piercing screams equally express an agony of pain.
- Laughter maybe either high or low; so that, with adult men, as Haller
- long ago remarked,[406] the sound partakes of the character of the
- vowels (as pronounced in German) _O_ and _A_; whilst with children and
- women, it has more of the character of _E_ and _I_; and these latter
- vowel-sounds naturally have, as Helmholtz has shown, a higher pitch
- than the former; yet both tones of laughter equally express enjoyment
- or amusement.
- In considering the mode in which vocal utterances express emotion, we
- are naturally led to inquire into the cause of what is called
- “expression” in music. Upon this point Mr. Litchfield, who has long
- attended to the subject of music, has been so kind as to give me the
- following remarks:—“The question, what is the essence of musical
- ‘expression’ involves a number of obscure points, which, so far as I am
- aware, are as yet unsolved enigmas. Up to a certain point, however, any
- law which is found to hold as to the expression of the emotions by
- simple sounds must apply to the more developed mode of expression in
- song, which may be taken as the primary type of all music. A great part
- of the emotional effect of a song depends on the character of the
- action by which the sounds are produced. In songs, for instance, which
- express great vehemence of passion, the effect often chiefly depends on
- the forcible utterance of some one or two characteristic passages which
- demand great exertion of vocal force; and it will be frequently noticed
- that a song of this character fails of its proper effect when sung by a
- voice of sufficient power and range to give the characteristic passages
- without much exertion. This is, no doubt, the secret of the loss of
- effect so often produced by the transposition of a song from one key to
- another. The effect is thus seen to depend not merely on the actual
- sounds, but also in part on the nature of the action which produces the
- sounds. Indeed it is obvious that whenever we feel the ‘expression’ of
- a song to be due to its quickness or slowness of movement—to smoothness
- of flow, loudness of utterance, and so on—we are, in fact, interpreting
- the muscular actions which produce sound, in the same way in which we
- interpret muscular action generally. But this leaves unexplained the
- more subtle and more specific effect which we call the _musical_
- expression of the song—the delight given by its melody, or even by the
- separate sounds which make up the melody. This is an effect indefinable
- in language—one which, so far as I am aware, no one has been able to
- analyse, and which the ingenious speculation of Mr. Herbert Spencer as
- to the origin of music leaves quite unexplained. For it is certain that
- the _melodic_ effect of a series of sounds does not depend in the least
- on their loudness or softness, or on their _absolute_ pitch. A tune is
- always the same tune, whether it is sung loudly or softly, by a child
- or a man; whether it is played on a flute or on a trombone. The purely
- musical effect of any sound depends on its place in what is technically
- called a ‘scale;’ the same sound producing absolutely different effects
- on the ear, according as it is heard in connection with one or another
- series of sounds.
- “It is on this _relative_ association of the sounds that all the
- essentially characteristic effects which are summed up in the phrase
- ‘musical expression,’ depend. But why certain associations of sounds
- have such-and-such effects, is a problem which yet remains to be
- solved. These effects must indeed, in some way or other, be connected
- with the well-known arithmetical relations between the rates of
- vibration of the sounds which form a musical scale. And it is
- possible—but this is merely a suggestion—that the greater or less
- mechanical facility with which the vibrating apparatus of the human
- larynx passes from one state of vibration to another, may have been a
- primary cause of the greater or less pleasure produced by various
- sequences of sounds.”
- But leaving aside these complex questions and confining ourselves to
- the simpler sounds, we can, at least, see some reasons for the
- association of certain kinds of sounds with certain states of mind. A
- scream, for instance, uttered by a young animal, or by one of the
- members of a community, as a call for assistance, will naturally be
- loud, prolonged, and high, so as to penetrate to a distance. For
- Helmholtz has shown[407] that, owing to the shape of the internal
- cavity of the human ear and its consequent power of resonance, high
- notes produce a particularly strong impression. When male animals utter
- sounds in order to please the females, they would naturally employ
- those which are sweet to the ears of the species; and it appears that
- the same sounds are often pleasing to widely different animals, owing
- to the similarity of their nervous systems, as we ourselves perceive in
- the singing of birds and even in the chirping of certain tree-frogs
- giving us pleasure. On the other hand, sounds produced in order to
- strike terror into an enemy, would naturally be harsh or displeasing.
- Whether the principle of antithesis has come into play with sounds, as
- might perhaps have been expected, is doubtful. The interrupted,
- laughing or tittering sounds made by man and by various kinds of
- monkeys when pleased, are as different as possible from the prolonged
- screams of these animals when distressed. The deep grunt of
- satisfaction uttered by a pig, when pleased with its food, is widely
- different from its harsh scream of pain or terror. But with the dog, as
- lately remarked, the bark of anger and that of joy are sounds which by
- no means stand in opposition to each other; and so it is in some other
- cases.
- There is another obscure point, namely, whether the sounds which are
- produced under various states of the mind determine the shape of the
- mouth, or whether its shape is not determined by independent causes,
- and the sound thus modified. When young infants cry they open their
- mouths widely, and this, no doubt, is necessary for pouring forth a
- full volume of sound; but the mouth then assumes, from a quite distinct
- cause, an almost quadrangular shape, depending, as will hereafter be
- explained, on the firm closing of the eyelids, and consequent drawing
- up of the upper lip. How far this square shape of the mouth modifies
- the wailing or crying sound, I am not prepared to say; but we know from
- the researches of Helmholtz and others that the form of the cavity of
- the mouth and lips determines the nature and pitch of the vowel sounds
- which are produced.
- It will also be shown in a future chapter that, under the feeling of
- contempt or disgust, there is a tendency, from intelligible causes, to
- blow out of the mouth or nostrils, and this produces sounds like pooh
- or pish. When any one is startled or suddenly astonished, there is an
- instantaneous tendency, likewise from an intelligible cause, namely, to
- be ready for prolonged exertion, to open the mouth widely, so as to
- draw a deep and rapid inspiration. When the next full expiration
- follows, the mouth is slightly closed, and the lips, from causes
- hereafter to be discussed, are somewhat protruded; and this form of the
- mouth, if the voice be at all exerted, produces, according to
- Helmholtz, the sound of the vowel _O_. Certainly a deep sound of a
- prolonged _Oh!_ may be heard from a whole crowd of people immediately
- after witnessing any astonishing spectacle. If, together with surprise,
- pain be felt, there is a tendency to contract all the muscles of the
- body, including those of the face, and the lips will then be drawn
- back; and this will perhaps account for the sound becoming higher and
- assuming the character of _Ah!_ or _Ach!_ As fear causes all the
- muscles of the body to tremble, the voice naturally becomes tremulous,
- and at the same time husky from the dryness of the mouth, owing to the
- salivary glands failing to act. Why the laughter of man and the
- tittering of monkeys should be a rapidly reiterated sound, cannot be
- explained. During the utterance of these sounds, the mouth is
- transversely elongated by the corners being drawn backwards and
- upwards; and of this fact an explanation will be attempted in a future
- chapter. But the whole subject of the differences of the sounds
- produced under different states of the mind is so obscure, that I have
- succeeded in throwing hardly any light on it; and the remarks which I
- have made, have but little significance.
- Sound Producing Quills from Tail of a Porcupine. Fig. 11
- All the sounds hitherto noticed depend on the respiratory organs; but
- sounds produced by wholly different means are likewise expressive.
- Rabbits stamp loudly on the ground as a signal to their comrades; and
- if a man knows how to do so properly, he may on a quiet evening hear
- the rabbits answering him all around. These animals, as well as some
- others, also stamp on the ground when made angry. Porcupines rattle
- their quills and vibrate their tails when angered; and one behaved in
- this manner when a live snake was placed in its compartment. The quills
- on the tail are very different from those on the body: they are short,
- hollow, thin like a goose-quill, with their ends transversely
- truncated, so that they are open; they are supported on long, thin,
- elastic foot-stalks. Now, when the tail is rapidly shaken, these hollow
- quills strike against each other and produce, as I heard in the
- presence of Mr. Bartlett, a peculiar continuous sound. We can, I think,
- understand why porcupines have been provided, through the modification
- of their protective spines, with this special sound-producing
- instrument. They are nocturnal animals, and if they scented or heard a
- prowling beast of prey, it would be a great advantage to them in the
- dark to give warning to their enemy what they were, and that they were
- furnished with dangerous spines. They would thus escape being attacked.
- They are, as I may add, so fully conscious of the power of their
- weapons, that when enraged they will charge backwards with their spines
- erected, yet still inclined backwards.
- Many birds during their courtship produce diversified sounds by means
- of specially adapted feathers. Storks, when excited, make a loud
- clattering noise with their beaks. Some snakes produce a grating or
- rattling noise. Many insects stridulate by rubbing together specially
- modified parts of their hard integuments. This stridulation generally
- serves as a sexual charm or call; but it is likewise used to express
- different emotions.[408] Every one who has attended to bees knows that
- their humming changes when they are angry; and this serves as a warning
- that there is danger of being stung. I have made these few remarks
- because some writers have laid so much stress on the vocal and
- respiratory organs as having been specially adapted for expression,
- that it was advisable to show that sounds otherwise produced serve
- equally well for the same purpose.
- _Erection of the dermal appendages_.—Hardly any expressive movement is
- so general as the involuntary erection of the hairs, feathers and other
- dermal appendages; for it is common throughout three of the great
- vertebrate classes. These appendages are erected under the excitement
- of anger or terror; more especially when these emotions are combined,
- or quickly succeed each other. The action serves to make the animal
- appear larger and more frightful to its enemies or rivals, and is
- generally accompanied by various voluntary movements adapted for the
- same purpose, and by the utterance of savage sounds. Mr. Bartlett, who
- has had such wide experience with animals of all kinds, does not doubt
- that this is the case; but it is a different question whether the power
- of erection was primarily acquired for this special purpose.
- I will first give a considerable body of facts showing how general this
- action is with mammals, birds and reptiles; retaining what I have to
- say in regard to man for a future chapter. Mr. Sutton, the intelligent
- keeper in the Zoological Gardens, carefully observed for me the
- Chimpanzee and Orang; and he states that when they are suddenly
- frightened, as by a thunderstorm, or when they are made angry, as by
- being teased, their hair becomes erect. I saw a chimpanzee who was
- alarmed at the sight of a black coalheaver, and the hair rose all over
- his body; he made little starts forward as if to attack the man,
- without any real intention of doing so, but with the hope, as the
- keeper remarked, of frightening him. The Gorilla, when enraged, is
- described by Mr. Ford[409] as having his crest of hair “erect and
- projecting forward, his nostrils dilated, and his under lip thrown
- down; at the same time uttering his characteristic yell, designed, it
- would seem, to terrify his antagonists.” I saw the hair on the Anubis
- baboon, when angered bristling along the back, from the neck to the
- loins, but not on the rump or other parts of the body. I took a stuffed
- snake into the monkey-house, and the hair on several of the species
- instantly became erect; especially on their tails, as I particularly
- noticed with the _Cereopithecus nictitans_. Brehm states[410] that the
- _Midas œdipus_ (belonging to the American division) when excited erects
- its mane, in order, as he adds, to make itself as frightful as
- possible.
- With the Carnivora the erection of the hair seems to be almost
- universal, often accompanied by threatening movements, the uncovering
- of the teeth and the utterance of savage growls. In the Herpestes, I
- have seen the hair on end over nearly the whole body, including the
- tail; and the dorsal crest is erected in a conspicuous manner by the
- Hyaena and Proteles. The enraged lion erects his mane. The bristling of
- the hair along the neck and back of the dog, and over the whole body of
- the cat, especially on the tail, is familiar to every one. With the cat
- it apparently occurs only under fear; with the dog, under anger and
- fear; but not, as far as I have observed, under abject fear, as when a
- dog is going to be flogged by a severe gamekeeper. If, however, the dog
- shows fight, as sometimes happens, up goes his hair. I have often
- noticed that the hair of a dog is particularly liable to rise, if he is
- half angry and half afraid, as on beholding some object only
- indistinctly seen in the dusk.
- I have been assured by a veterinary surgeon that he has often seen the
- hair erected on horses and cattle, on which he had operated and was
- again going to operate. When I showed a stuffed snake to a Peccary, the
- hair rose in a wonderful manner along its back; and so it does with the
- boar when enraged. An Elk which gored a man to death in the United
- States, is described as first brandishing his antlers, squealing with
- rage and stamping on the ground; “at length his hair was seen to rise
- and stand on end,” and then he plunged forward to the attack.[411] The
- hair likewise becomes erect on goats, and, as I hear from Mr. Blyth, on
- some Indian antelopes. I have seen it erected on the hairy Ant-eater;
- and on the Agouti, one of the Rodents. A female Bat,[412] which reared
- her young under confinement, when any one looked into the cage “erected
- the fur on her back, and bit viciously at intruding fingers.”
- Birds belonging to all the chief Orders ruffle their feathers when
- angry or frightened. Every one must have seen two cocks, even quite
- young birds, preparing to fight with erected neck-hackles; nor can
- these feathers when erected serve as a means of defence, for
- cock-fighters have found by experience that it is advantageous to trim
- them. The male Ruff (_Machetes pugnæ_) likewise erects its collar of
- feathers when fighting. When a dog approaches a common hen with her
- chickens, she spreads out her wings, raises her tail, ruffles all her
- feathers, and looking as ferocious as possible, dashes at the intruder.
- The tail is not always held in exactly the same position; it is
- sometimes so much erected, that the central feathers, as in the
- accompanying drawing, almost touch the back. Swans, when angered,
- likewise raise their wings and tail, and erect their feathers. They
- open their beaks, and make by paddling little rapid starts forwards,
- against any one who approaches the water’s edge too closely. Tropic
- birds[413] when disturbed on their nests are said not to fly away, but
- “merely to stick out their feathers and scream.” The Barn-owl, when
- approached “instantly swells out its plumage, extends its wings and
- tail, hisses and clacks its mandibles with force and rapidity.”[414] So
- do other kinds of owls. Hawks, as I am informed by Mr. Jenner Weir,
- likewise ruffle their feathers, and spread out their wings and tail
- under similar circumstances. Some kinds of parrots erect their
- feathers; and I have seen this action in the Cassowary, when angered at
- the sight of an Ant-eater. Young cuckoos in the nest, raise their
- feathers, open their mouths widely, and make themselves as frightful as
- possible.
- Hen Driving Away a Dog from Her Chickens. Fig. 12
- {illust. caption = FIG. 12—Hen driving away a dog from her chickens.
- Drawn from life by Mr. Wood.}
- Swan Driving Away an Intruder. Fig 13
- {illust. caption = FIG. 13.—Swan driving away an intruder. Drawn from
- life by Mr. Wood.}
- Small birds, also, as I hear from Mr. Weir, such as various finches,
- buntings and warblers, when angry, ruffle all their feathers, or only
- those round the neck; or they spread out their wings and tail-feathers.
- With their plumage in this state, they rush at each other with open
- beaks and threatening gestures. Mr. Weir concludes from his large
- experience that the erection of the feathers is caused much more by
- anger than by fear. He gives as an instance a hybrid goldfinch of a
- most irascible disposition, which when approached too closely by a
- servant, instantly assumes the appearance of a ball of ruffled
- feathers. He believes that birds when frightened, as a general rule,
- closely adpress all their feathers, and their consequently diminished
- size is often astonishing. As soon as they recover from their fear or
- surprise, the first thing which they do is to shake out their feathers.
- The best instances of this adpression of the feathers and apparent
- shrinking of the body from fear, which Mr. Weir has noticed, has been
- in the quail and grass-parrakeet.[415] The habit is intelligible in
- these birds from their being accustomed, when in danger, either to
- squat on the ground or to sit motionless on a branch, so as to escape
- detection. Though, with birds, anger may be the chief and commonest
- cause of the erection of the feathers, it is probable that young
- cuckoos when looked at in the nest, and a hen with her chickens when
- approached by a dog, feel at least some terror. Mr. Tegetmeier informs
- me that with game-cocks, the erection of the feathers on the head has
- long been recognized in the cock-pit as a sign of cowardice.
- The males of some lizards, when fighting together during their
- courtship, expand their throat pouches or frills, and erect their
- dorsal crests.[416] But Dr. Günther does not believe that they can
- erect their separate spines or scales.
- We thus see how generally throughout the two higher vertebrate classes,
- and with some reptiles, the dermal appendages are erected under the
- influence of anger and fear. The movement is effected, as we know from
- Kolliker’s interesting discovery, by the contraction of minute,
- unstriped, involuntary muscles,[417] often called _arrectores pili_,
- which are attached to the capsules of the separate hairs, feathers, &c.
- By the contraction of these muscles the hairs can be instantly erected,
- as we see in a dog, being at the same time drawn a little out of their
- sockets; they are afterwards quickly depressed. The vast number of
- these minute muscles over the whole body of a hairy quadruped is
- astonishing. The erection of the hair is, however, aided in some cases,
- as with that on the head of a man, by the striped and voluntary muscles
- of the underlying _panniculus carnosus_. It is by the action of these
- latter muscles, that the hedgehog erects its spines. It appears, also,
- from the researches of Leydig[418] and others, that striped fibres
- extend from the panniculus to some of the larger hairs, such as the
- vibrissae of certain quadrupeds. The _arrectores pili_ contract not
- only under the above emotions, but from the application of cold to the
- surface. I remember that my mules and dogs, brought from a lower and
- warmer country, after spending a night on the bleak Cordillera, had the
- hair all over their bodies as erect as under the greatest terror. We
- see the same action in our own _goose-skin_ during the chill before a
- fever-fit. Mr. Lister has also found,[419] that tickling a neighbouring
- part of the skin causes the erection and protrusion of the hairs.
- From these facts it is manifest that the erection of the dermal
- appendages is a reflex action, independent of the will; and this action
- must be looked at, when, occurring under the influence of anger or
- fear, not as a power acquired for the sake of some advantage, but as an
- incidental result, at least to a large extent, of the sensorium being
- affected. The result, in as far as it is incidental, may be compared
- with the profuse sweating from an agony of pain or terror.
- Nevertheless, it is remarkable how slight an excitement often suffices
- to cause the hair to become erect; as when two dogs pretend to fight
- together in play. We have, also, seen in a large number of animals,
- belonging to widely distinct classes, that the erection of the hair or
- feathers is almost always accompanied by various voluntary movements—by
- threatening gestures, opening the mouth, uncovering the teeth,
- spreading out of the wings and tail by birds, and by the utterance of
- harsh sounds; and the purpose of these voluntary movements is
- unmistakable. Therefore it seems hardly credible that the co-ordinated
- erection of the dermal appendages, by which the animal is made to
- appear larger and more terrible to its enemies or rivals, should be
- altogether an incidental and purposeless result of the disturbance of
- the sensorium. This seems almost as incredible as that the erection by
- the hedgehog of its spines, or of the quills by the porcupine, or of
- the ornamental plumes by many birds during their courtship, should all
- be purposeless actions.
- We here encounter a great difficulty. How can the contraction of the
- unstriped and involuntary _arrectores pili_ have been co-ordinated with
- that of various voluntary muscles for the same special purpose? If we
- could believe that the arrectores primordially had been voluntary
- muscles, and had since lost their stripes and become involuntary, the
- case would be comparatively simple. I am not, however, aware that there
- is any evidence in favour of this view; although the reversed
- transition would not have presented any great difficulty, as the
- voluntary muscles are in an unstriped condition in the embryos of the
- higher animals, and in the larvae of some crustaceans. Moreover in the
- deeper layers of the skin of adult birds, the muscular network is,
- according to Leydig,[420] in a transitional condition; the fibres
- exhibiting only indications of transverse striation.
- Another explanation seems possible. We may admit that originally the
- _arrectores pili_ were slightly acted on in a direct manner, under the
- influence of rage and terror, by the disturbance of the nervous system;
- as is undoubtedly the case with our so-called _goose-skin_ before a
- fever-fit. Animals have been repeatedly excited by rage and terror
- during many generations; and consequently the direct effects of the
- disturbed nervous system on the dermal appendages will almost certainly
- have been increased through habit and through the tendency of
- nerve-force to pass readily along accustomed channels. We shall find
- this view of the force of habit strikingly confirmed in a future
- chapter, where it will be shown that the hair of the insane is affected
- in an extraordinary manner, owing to their repeated accesses of fury
- and terror. As soon as with animals the power of erection had thus been
- strengthened or increased, they must often have seen the hairs or
- feathers erected in rival and enraged males, and the bulk of their
- bodies thus increased. In this case it appears possible that they might
- have wished to make themselves appear larger and more terrible to their
- enemies, by voluntarily assuming a threatening attitude and uttering
- harsh cries; such attitudes and utterances after a time becoming
- through habit instinctive. In this manner actions performed by the
- contraction of voluntary muscles might have been combined for the same
- special purpose with those effected by involuntary muscles. It is even
- possible that animals, when excited and dimly conscious of some change
- in the state of their hair, might act on it by repeated exertions of
- their attention and will; for we have reason to believe that the will
- is able to influence in an obscure manner the action of some unstriped
- or involuntary muscles, as in the period of the peristaltic movements
- of the intestines, and in the contraction of the bladder. Nor must we
- overlook the part which variation and natural selection may have
- played; for the males which succeeded in making themselves appear the
- most terrible to their rivals, or to their other enemies, if not of
- overwhelming power, will on an average have left more offspring to
- inherit their characteristic qualities, whatever these may be and
- however first acquired, than have other males.
- _The inflation of the body, and other means of exciting fear in an
- enemy_.—Certain Amphibians and Reptiles, which either have no spines to
- erect, or no muscles by which they can be erected, enlarge themselves
- when alarmed or angry by inhaling air. This is well known to be the
- case with toads and frogs. The latter animal is made, in AEsop’s fable
- of the ‘Ox and the Frog,’ to blow itself up from vanity and envy until
- it burst. This action must have been observed during the most ancient
- times, as, according to Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood,[421] the word _toad_
- expresses in all the languages of Europe the habit of swelling. It has
- been observed with some of the exotic species in the Zoological
- Gardens; and Dr. Günther believes that it is general throughout the
- group. Judging from analogy, the primary purpose probably was to make
- the body appear as large and frightful as possible to an enemy; but
- another, and perhaps more important secondary advantage is thus gained.
- When frogs are seized by snakes, which are their chief enemies, they
- enlarge themselves wonderfully; so that if the snake be of small size,
- as Dr. Günther informs me, it cannot swallow the frog, which thus
- escapes being devoured.
- Chameleons and some other lizards inflate themselves when angry. Thus a
- species inhabiting Oregon, the _Tapaya Douglasii_, is slow in its
- movements and does not bite, but has a ferocious aspect; “when
- irritated it springs in a most threatening manner at anything pointed
- at it, at the same time opening its mouth wide and hissing audibly,
- after which it inflates its body, and shows other marks of anger.”[422]
- Several kinds of snakes likewise inflate themselves when irritated. The
- puff-adder (_Clotho arietans_) is remarkable in this respect; but I
- believe, after carefully watching these animals, that they do not act
- thus for the sake of increasing their apparent bulk, but simply for
- inhaling a large supply of air, so as to produce their surprisingly
- loud, harsh, and prolonged hissing sound. The Cobras-de-capello, when
- irritated, enlarge themselves a little, and hiss moderately; but, at
- the same time they lift their heads aloft, and dilate by means of their
- elongated anterior ribs, the skin on each side of the neck into a large
- flat disk,—the so-called hood. With their widely opened mouths, they
- then assume a terrific aspect. The benefit thus derived ought to be
- considerable, in order to compensate for the somewhat lessened rapidity
- (though this is still great) with which, when dilated, they can strike
- at their enemies or prey; on the same principle that a broad, thin
- piece of wood cannot be moved through the air so quickly as a small
- round stick. An innocuous snake, the _Trovidonotus macrophthalmus_, an
- inhabitant of India, likewise dilates its neck when irritated; and
- consequently is often mistaken for its compatriot, the deadly
- Cobra.[423] This resemblance perhaps serves as some protection to the
- Tropidonotus. Another innocuous species, the Dasypeltis of South
- Africa, blows itself out, distends its neck, hisses and darts at an
- intruder.[424] Many other snakes hiss under similar circumstances. They
- also rapidly vibrate their protruded tongues; and this may aid in
- increasing their terrific appearance.
- Snakes possess other means of producing sounds besides hissing. Many
- years ago I observed in South America that a venomous Trigonocephalus,
- when disturbed, rapidly vibrated the end of its tail, which striking
- against the dry grass and twigs produced a rattling noise that could be
- distinctly heard at the distance of six feet.[425] The deadly and
- fierce _Echis carinata_ of India produces “a curious prolonged, almost
- hissing sound in a very different manner, namely by rubbing the sides
- of the folds of its body against each other,” whilst the head remains
- in almost the same position. The scales on the sides, and not on other
- parts of the body, are strongly keeled, with the keels toothed like a
- saw; and as the coiled-up animal rubs its sides together, these grate
- against each other.[426] Lastly, we have the well-known case of the
- Rattle-snake. He who has merely shaken the rattle of a dead snake, can
- form no just idea of the sound produced by the living animal. Professor
- Shaler states that it is indistinguishable from that made by the male
- of a large Cicada (an Homopterous insect), which inhabits the same
- district.[427] In the Zoological Gardens, when the rattle-snakes and
- puff-adders were greatly excited at the same time, I was much struck at
- the similarity of the sound produced by them; and although that made by
- the rattle-snake is louder and shriller than the hissing of the
- puff-adder, yet when standing at some yards distance I could scarcely
- distinguish the two. For whatever purpose the sound is produced by the
- one species, I can hardly doubt that it serves for the same purpose in
- the other species; and I conclude from the threatening gestures made at
- the same time by many snakes, that their hissing,—the rattling of the
- rattle-snake and of the tail of the Trigonocephalus,—the grating of the
- scales of the Echis,—and the dilatation of the hood of the Cobra,—all
- subserve the same end, namely, to make them appear terrible to their
- enemies.[428]
- It seems at first a probable conclusion that venomous snakes, such as
- the foregoing, from being already so well defended by their
- poison-fangs, would never be attacked by any enemy; and consequently
- would have no need to excite additional terror. But this is far from
- being the case, for they are largely preyed on in all quarters of the
- world by many animals. It is well known that pigs are employed in the
- United States to clear districts infested with rattle-snakes, which
- they do most effectually.[429] In England the hedgehog attacks and
- devours the viper. In India, as I hear from Dr. Jerdon, several kinds
- of hawks, and at least one mammal, the Herpestes, kill cobras and other
- venomous species;[430] and so it is in South Africa. Therefore it is by
- no means improbable that any sounds or signs by which the venomous
- species could instantly make themselves recognized as dangerous, would
- be of more service to them than to the innocuous species which would
- not be able, if attacked, to inflict any real injury.
- Having said thus much about snakes, I am tempted to add a few remarks
- on the means by which the rattle of the rattle-snake was probably
- developed. Various animals, including some lizards, either curl or
- vibrate their tails when excited. This is the case with many kinds of
- snakes.[431] In the Zoological Gardens, an innocuous species, the
- _Coronella Sayi_, vibrates its tail so rapidly that it becomes almost
- invisible. The Trigonocephalus, before alluded to, has the same habit;
- and the extremity of its tail is a little enlarged, or ends in a bead.
- In the Lachesis, which is so closely allied to the rattle-snake that it
- was placed by Linnaeus in the same genus, the tail ends in a single,
- large, lancet-shaped point or scale. With some snakes the skin, as
- Professor Shaler remarks, “is more imperfectly detached from the region
- about the tail than at other parts of the body.” Now if we suppose that
- the end of the tail of some ancient American species was enlarged, and
- was covered by a single large scale, this could hardly have been cast
- off at the successive moults. In this case it would have been
- permanently retained, and at each period of growth, as the snake grew
- larger, a new scale, larger than the last, would have been formed above
- it, and would likewise have been retained. The foundation for the
- development of a rattle would thus have been laid; and it would have
- been habitually used, if the species, like so many others, vibrated its
- tail whenever it was irritated. That the rattle has since been
- specially developed to serve as an efficient sound-producing
- instrument, there can hardly be a doubt; for even the vertebrae
- included within the extremity of the tail have been altered in shape
- and cohere. But there is no greater improbability in various
- structures, such as the rattle of the rattle-snake,—the lateral scales
- of the Echis,—the neck with the included ribs of the Cobra,—and the
- whole body of the puff-adder,—having been modified for the sake of
- warning and frightening away their enemies, than in a bird, namely, the
- wonderful Secretary-hawk (_Gypogeranus_) having had its whole frame
- modified for the sake of killing snakes with impunity. It is highly
- probable, judging from what we have before seen, that this bird would
- ruffle its feathers whenever it attacked a snake; and it is certain
- that the Herpestes, when it eagerly rushes to attack a snake, erects
- the hair all over its body, and especially that on its tail.[432] We
- have also seen that some porcupines, when angered or alarmed at the
- sight of a snake, rapidly vibrate their tails, thus producing a
- peculiar sound by the striking together of the hollow quills. So that
- here both the attackers and the attacked endeavour to make themselves
- as dreadful as possible to each other; and both possess for this
- purpose specialised means, which, oddly enough, are nearly the same in
- some of these cases. Finally we can see that if, on the one hand, those
- individual snakes, which were best able to frighten away their enemies,
- escaped best from being devoured; and if, on the other hand, those
- individuals of the attacking enemy survived in larger numbers which
- were the best fitted for the dangerous task of killing and devouring
- venomous snakes;—then in the one case as in the other, beneficial
- variations, supposing the characters in question to vary, would
- commonly have been preserved through the survival of the fittest.
- _The Drawing back and pressure of the Ears to the Head_.—The ears
- through their movements are highly expressive in many animals; but in
- some, such as man, the higher apes, and many ruminants, they fail in
- this respect. A slight difference in position serves to express in the
- plainest manner a different state of mind, as we may daily see in the
- dog; but we are here concerned only with the ears being drawn closely
- backwards and pressed to the head. A savage frame of mind is thus
- shown, but only in the case of those animals which fight with their
- teeth; and the care which they take to prevent their ears being seized
- by their antagonists, accounts for this position. Consequently, through
- habit and association, whenever they feel slightly savage, or pretend
- in their play to be savage, their ears are drawn back. That this is the
- true explanation may be inferred from the relation which exists in very
- many animals between their manner of fighting and the retraction of
- their ears.
- All the Carnivora fight with their canine teeth, and all, as far as I
- have observed, draw their ears back when feeling savage. This may be
- continually seen with dogs when fighting in earnest, and with puppies
- fighting in play. The movement is different from the falling down and
- slight drawing back of the ears, when a dog feels pleased and is
- caressed by his master. The retraction of the ears may likewise be seen
- in kittens fighting together in their play, and in full-grown cats when
- really savage, as before illustrated in fig. 9 (p. 58). Although their
- ears are thus to a large extent protected, yet they often get much torn
- in old male cats during their mutual battles. The same movement is very
- striking in tigers, leopards, &c., whilst growling over their food in
- menageries. The lynx has remarkably long ears; and their retraction,
- when one of these animals is approached in its cage, is very
- conspicuous, and is eminently expressive of its savage disposition.
- Even one of the Eared Seals, the _Otariapusilla_, which has very small
- ears, draws them backwards, when it makes a savage rush at the legs of
- its keeper.
- When horses fight together they use their incisors for biting, and
- their fore-legs for striking, much more than they do their hind-legs
- for kicking backwards. This has been observed when stallions have
- broken loose and have fought together, and may likewise be inferred
- from the kind of wounds which they inflict on each other. Every one
- recognizes the vicious appearance which the drawing back of the ears
- gives to a horse. This movement is very different from that of
- listening to a sound behind. If an ill-tempered horse in a stall is
- inclined to kick backwards, his ears are retracted from habit, though
- he has no intention or power to bite. But when a horse throws up both
- hind-legs in play, as when entering an open field, or when just touched
- by the whip, he does not generally depress his ears, for he does not
- then feel vicious. Guanacoes fight savagely with their teeth; and they
- must do so frequently, for I found the hides of several which I shot in
- Patagonia deeply scored. So do camels; and both these animals, when
- savage, draw their ears closely backwards. Guanacoes, as I have
- noticed, when not intending to bite, but merely to spit their offensive
- saliva from a distance at an intruder, retract their ears. Even the
- hippopotamus, when threatening with its widely-open enormous mouth a
- comrade, draws back its small ears, just like a horse.
- Now what a contrast is presented between the foregoing animals and
- cattle, sheep, or goats, which never use their teeth in fighting, and
- never draw back their ears when enraged! Although sheep and goats
- appear such placid animals, the males often join in furious contests.
- As deer form a closely related family, and as I did not know that they
- ever fought with their teeth, I was much surprised at the account given
- by Major Ross King of the Moose-deer in Canada. He says, when“two males
- chance to meet, laying back their ears and gnashing their teeth
- together, they rush at each other with appalling fury.”[433] But Mr.
- Bartlett informs me that some species of deer fight savagely with their
- teeth, so that the drawing back of the ears by the moose accords with
- our rule. Several kinds of kangaroos, kept in the Zoological Gardens,
- fight by scratching with their fore-feet and by kicking with their
- hind-legs; but they never bite each other, and the keepers have never
- seen them draw back their ears when angered. Rabbits fight chiefly by
- kicking and scratching, but they likewise bite each other; and I have
- known one to bite off half the tail of its antagonist. At the
- commencement of their battles they lay back their ears, but afterwards,
- as they bound over and kick each other, they keep their ears erect, or
- move them much about.
- Mr. Bartlett watched a wild boar quarrelling rather savagely with his
- sow; and both had their mouths open and their ears drawn backwards. But
- this does not appear to be a common action with domestic pigs when
- quarrelling. Boars fight together by striking upwards with their tusks;
- and Mr. Bartlett doubts whether they then draw back their ears.
- Elephants, which in like manner fight with their tusks, do not retract
- their ears, but, on the contrary, erect them when rushing at each other
- or at an enemy.
- The rhinoceroses in the Zoological Gardens fight with their nasal
- horns, and have never been seen to attempt biting each other except in
- play; and the keepers are convinced that they do not draw back their
- ears, like horses and dogs, when feeling savage. The following
- statement, therefore, by Sir S. Baker[434] is inexplicable, namely,
- that a rhinoceros, which he shot in North Africa, “had no ears; they
- had been bitten off close to the head by another of the same species
- while fighting; and this mutilation is by no means uncommon.”
- Lastly, with respect to monkeys. Some kinds, which have moveable ears,
- and which fight with their teeth—for instance the _Cereopithecus
- ruber_—draw back their ears when irritated just like dogs; and they
- then have a very spiteful appearance. Other kinds, as the _Inuus
- ecaudatus_, apparently do not thus act. Again, other kinds—and this is
- a great anomaly in comparison with most other animals—retract their
- ears, show their teeth, and jabber, when they are pleased by being
- caressed. I observed this in two or three species of Macacus, and in
- the _Cynopithecus niger_. This expression, owing to our familiarity
- with dogs, would never be recognized as one of joy or pleasure by those
- unacquainted with monkeys.
- _Erection of the Ears_.—This movement requires hardly any notice. All
- animals which have the power of freely moving their ears, when they are
- startled, or when they closely observe any object, direct their ears to
- the point towards which they are looking, in order to hear any sound
- from this quarter. At the same time they generally raise their heads,
- as all their organs of sense are there situated, and some of the
- smaller animals rise on their hind-legs. Even those kinds which squat
- on the ground or instantly flee away to avoid danger, generally act
- momentarily in this manner, in order to ascertain the source and nature
- of the danger. The head being raised, with erected ears and eyes
- directed forwards, gives an unmistakable expression of close attention
- to any animal.
- CHAPTER V. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF ANIMALS.
- The Dog, various expressive movements of—Cats—Horses—Ruminants—Monkeys,
- their expression of joy and affection—Of pain—Anger—Astonishment and
- Terror.
- _The Dog_.—I have already described (figs. 5 and 7) the appearance of a
- dog approaching another dog with hostile intentions, namely, with
- erected ears, eyes intently directed forwards, hair on the neck and
- back bristling, gait remarkably stiff, with the tail upright and rigid.
- So familiar is this appearance to us, that an angry man is sometimes
- said “to have his back up.” Of the above points, the stiff gait and
- upright tail alone require further discussion. Sir C. Bell remarks[501]
- that, when a tiger or wolf is struck by its keeper and is suddenly
- roused to ferocity, every muscle is in tension, and the limbs are in an
- attitude of strained exertion, prepared to spring. This tension of the
- muscles and consequent stiff gait may be accounted for on the principle
- of associated habit, for anger has continually led to fierce struggles,
- and consequently to all the muscles of the body having been violently
- exerted. There is also reason to suspect that the muscular system
- requires some short preparation, or some degree of innervation, before
- being brought into strong action. My own sensations lead me to this
- inference; but I cannot discover that it is a conclusion admitted by
- physiologists. Sir J. Paget, however, informs me that when muscles are
- suddenly contracted with the greatest force, without any preparation,
- they are liable to be ruptured, as when a man slips unexpectedly; but
- that this rarely occurs when an action, however violent, is
- deliberately performed.
- With respect to the upright position of the tail, it seems to depend
- (but whether this is really the case I know not) on the elevator
- muscles being more powerful than the depressors, so that when all the
- muscles of the hinder part of the body are in a state of tension, the
- tail is raised. A dog in cheerful spirits, and trotting before his
- master with high, elastic steps, generally carries his tail aloft,
- though it is not held nearly so stiffly as when he is angered. A horse
- when first turned out into an open field, may be seen to trot with long
- elastic strides, the head and tail being held high aloft. Even cows
- when they frisk about from pleasure, throw up their tails in a
- ridiculous fashion. So it is with various animals in the Zoological
- Gardens. The position of the tail, however, in certain cases, is
- determined by special circumstances; thus as soon as a horse breaks
- into a gallop, at full speed, he always lowers his tail, so that as
- little resistance as possible may be offered to the air.
- When a dog is on the point of springing on his antagonist, he utters a
- savage growl; the ears are pressed closely backwards, and the upper lip
- (fig. 14) is retracted out of the way of his teeth, especially of his
- canines. These movements may be observed with dogs and puppies in their
- play. But if a dog gets really savage in his play, his expression
- immediately changes. This, however, is simply due to the lips and ears
- being drawn back with much greater energy. If a dog only snarls at
- another, the lip is generally retracted on one side alone, namely
- towards his enemy.
- Head of Snarling Dog. Fig 14
- {illust. caption = FIG. 14.—Head of snarling Dog. From life, by Mr.
- Wood.
- The movements of a dog whilst exhibiting affection towards his master
- were described (figs. 6 and 8) in our second chapter. These consist in
- the head and whole body being lowered and thrown into flexuous
- movements, with the tail extended and wagged from side to side. The
- ears fall down and are drawn somewhat backwards, which causes the
- eyelids to be elongated, and alters the whole appearance of the face.
- The lips hang loosely, and the hair remains smooth. All these movements
- or gestures are explicable, as I believe, from their standing in
- complete antithesis to those naturally assumed by a savage dog under a
- directly opposite state of mind. When a man merely speaks to, or just
- notices, his dog, we see the last vestige of these movements in a
- slight wag of the tail, without any other movement of the body, and
- without even the ears being lowered. Dogs also exhibit their affection
- by desiring to rub against their masters, and to be rubbed or patted by
- them.
- Gratiolet explains the above gestures of affection in the following
- manner: and the reader can judge whether the explanation appears
- satisfactory. Speaking of animals in general, including the dog, he
- says,[502] “C’est toujours la partie la plus sensible de leurs corps
- qui recherche les caresses ou les donne. Lorsque toute la longueur des
- flancs et du corps est sensible, l’animal serpente et rampe sous les
- caresses; et ces ondulations se propageant le long des muscles
- analogues des segments jusqu’aux extrémités de la colonne vertébrale,
- la queue se ploie et s’agite.” Further on, he adds, that dogs, when
- feeling affectionate, lower their ears in order to exclude all sounds,
- so that their whole attention may be concentrated on the caresses of
- their master!
- Dogs have another and striking way of exhibiting their affection,
- namely, by licking the hands or faces of their masters. They sometimes
- lick other dogs, and then it is always their chops. I have also seen
- dogs licking cats with whom they were friends. This habit probably
- originated in the females carefully licking their puppies—the dearest
- object of their love—for the sake of cleansing them. They also often
- give their puppies, after a short absence, a few cursory licks,
- apparently from affection. Thus the habit will have become associated
- with the emotion of love, however it may afterwards be aroused. It is
- now so firmly inherited or innate, that it is transmitted equally to
- both sexes. A female terrier of mine lately had her puppies destroyed,
- and though at all times a very affectionate creature, I was much struck
- with the manner in which she then tried to satisfy her instinctive
- maternal love by expending it on me; and her desire to lick my hands
- rose to an insatiable passion.
- The same principle probably explains why dogs, when feeling
- affectionate, like rubbing against their masters and being rubbed or
- patted by them, for from the nursing of their puppies, contact with a
- beloved object has become firmly associated in their minds with the
- emotion of love.
- The feeling of affection of a dog towards his master is combined with a
- strong sense of submission, which is akin to fear. Hence dogs not only
- lower their bodies and crouch a little as they approach their masters,
- but sometimes throw themselves on the ground with their bellies
- upwards. This is a movement as completely opposite as is possible to
- any show of resistance. I formerly possessed a large dog who was not at
- all afraid to fight with other dogs; but a wolf-like shepherd-dog in
- the neighbourhood, though not ferocious and not so powerful as my dog,
- had a strange influence over him. When they met on the road, my dog
- used to run to meet him, with his tail partly tucked in between his
- legs and hair not erected; and then he would throw himself on the
- ground, belly upwards. By this action he seemed to say more plainly
- than by words, “Behold, I am your slave.”
- A pleasurable and excited state of mind, associated with affection, is
- exhibited by some dogs in a very peculiar manner, namely, by grinning.
- This was noticed long ago by Somerville, who says,
- “And with a courtly grin, the fawning hound
- Salutes thee cow’ring, his wide op’ning nose
- Upward he curls, and his large sloe-back eyes
- Melt in soft blandishments, and humble joy.”
- _The Chase_, book i.
- Sir W. Scott’s famous Scotch greyhound, Maida, had this habit, and it
- is common with terriers. I have also seen it in a Spitz and in a
- sheep-dog. Mr. Riviere, who has particularly attended to this
- expression, informs me that it is rarely displayed in a perfect manner,
- but is quite common in a lesser degree. The upper lip during the act of
- grinning is retracted, as in snarling, so that the canines are exposed,
- and the ears are drawn backwards; but the general appearance of the
- animal clearly shows that anger is not felt. Sir C. Bell[503] remarks
- “Dogs, in their expression of fondness, have a slight eversion of the
- lips, and grin and sniff amidst their gambols, in a way that resembles
- laughter.” Some persons speak of the grin as a smile, but if it had
- been really a smile, we should see a similar, though more pronounced,
- movement of the lips and ears, when dogs utter their bark of joy; but
- this is not the case, although a bark of joy often follows a grin. On
- the other hand, dogs, when playing with their comrades or masters,
- almost always pretend to bite each other; and they then retract, though
- not energetically, their lips and ears. Hence I suspect that there is a
- tendency in some dogs, whenever they feel lively pleasure combined with
- affection, to act through habit and association on the same muscles, as
- in playfully biting each other, or their masters’ hands.
- I have described, in the second chapter, the gait and appearance of a
- dog when cheerful, and the marked antithesis presented by the same
- animal when dejected and disappointed, with his head, ears, body, tail,
- and chops drooping, and eyes dull. Under the expectation of any great
- pleasure, dogs bound and jump about in an extravagant manner, and bark
- for joy. The tendency to bark under this state of mind is inherited, or
- runs in the breed: greyhounds rarely bark, whilst the Spitz-dog barks
- so incessantly on starting for a walk with his master that he becomes a
- nuisance.
- An agony of pain is expressed by dogs in nearly the same way as by many
- other animals, namely, by howling writhing, and contortions of the
- whole body.
- Attention is shown by the head being raised, with the ears erected, and
- eyes intently directed towards the object or quarter under observation.
- If it be a sound and the source is not known, the head is often turned
- obliquely from side to side in a most significant manner, apparently in
- order to judge with more exactness from what point the sound proceeds.
- But I have seen a dog greatly surprised at a new noise, turning, his
- head to one side through habit, though he clearly perceived the source
- of the noise. Dogs, as formerly remarked, when their attention is in
- any way aroused, whilst watching some object, or attending to some
- sound, often lift up one paw (fig. 4) and keep it doubled up, as if to
- make a slow and stealthy approach.
- A dog under extreme terror will throw himself down, howl, and void his
- excretions; but the hair, I believe, does not become erect unless some
- anger is felt. I have seen a dog much terrified at a band of musicians
- who were playing loudly outside the house, with every muscle of his
- body trembling, with his heart palpitating so quickly that the beats
- could hardly be counted, and panting for breath with widely open mouth,
- in the same manner as a terrified man does. Yet this dog had not
- exerted himself; he had only wandered slowly and restlessly about the
- room, and the day was cold.
- Even a very slight degree of fear is invariably shown by the tail being
- tucked in between the legs. This tucking in of the fail is accompanied
- by the ears being drawn backwards; but they are not pressed closely to
- the head, as in snarling, and they are not lowered, as when a dog is
- pleased or affectionate. When two young dogs chase each other in play,
- the one that runs away always keeps his tail tucked inwards. So it is
- when a dog, in the highest spirits, careers like a mad creature round
- and round his master in circles, or in figures of eight. He then acts
- as if another dog were chasing him. This curious kind of play, which
- must be familiar to every one who has attended to dogs, is particularly
- apt to be excited, after the animal has been a little startled or
- frightened, as by his master suddenly jumping out on him in the dusk.
- In this case, as well as when two young dogs are chasing each other in
- play, it appears as if the one that runs away was afraid of the other
- catching him by the tail; but as far as I can find out, dogs very
- rarely catch each other in this manner. I asked a gentleman, who had
- kept foxhounds all his life, and he applied to other experienced
- sportsmen, whether they had ever seen hounds thus seize a fox; but they
- never had. It appears that when a dog is chased, or when in danger of
- being struck behind, or of anything falling on him, in all these cases
- he wishes to withdraw as quickly as possible his whole hind-quarters,
- and that from some sympathy or connection between the muscles, the tail
- is then drawn closely inwards.
- A similarly connected movement between the hind-quarters and the tail
- may be observed in the hyaena. Mr. Bartlett informs me that when two of
- these animals fight together, they are mutually conscious of the
- wonderful power of each other’s jaws, and are extremely cautious. They
- well know that if one of their legs were seized, the bone would
- instantly be crushed into atoms; hence they approach each other
- kneeling, with their legs turned as much as possible inwards, and with
- their whole bodies bowed, so as not to present any salient point; the
- tail at the same time being closely tucked in between the legs. In this
- attitude they approach each other sideways, or even partly backwards.
- So again with deer, several of the species, when savage and fighting,
- tuck in their tails. When one horse in a field tries to bite the
- hind-quarters of another in play, or when a rough boy strikes a donkey
- from behind, the hind-quarters and the tail are drawn in, though it
- does not appear as if this were done merely to save the tail from being
- injured. We have also seen the reverse of these movements; for when an
- animal trots with high elastic steps, the tail is almost always carried
- aloft.
- As I have said, when a dog is chased and runs away, he keeps his ears
- directed backwards but still open; and this is clearly done for the
- sake of hearing the footsteps of his pursuer. From habit the ears are
- often held in this same position, and the tail tucked in, when the
- danger is obviously in front. I have repeatedly noticed, with a timid
- terrier of mine, that when she is afraid of some object in front, the
- nature of which she perfectly knows and does not need to reconnoitre,
- yet she will for a long time hold her ears and tail in this position,
- looking the image of discomfort. Discomfort, without any fear, is
- similarly expressed: thus, one day I went out of doors, just at the
- time when this same dog knew that her dinner would be brought. I did
- not call her, but she wished much to accompany me, and at the same time
- she wished much for her dinner; and there she stood, first looking one
- way and then the other, with her tail tucked in and ears drawn back,
- presenting an unmistakable appearance of perplexed discomfort.
- Almost all the expressive movements now described, with the exception
- of the grinning from joy, are innate or instinctive, for they are
- common to all the individuals, young and old, of all the breeds. Most
- of them are likewise common to the aboriginal parents of the dog,
- namely the wolf and jackal; and some of them to other species of the
- same group. Tamed wolves and jackals, when caressed by their masters,
- jump about for joy, wag their tails, lower their ears, lick their
- master’s hands, crouch down, and even throw themselves on the ground
- belly upwards.[504] I have seen a rather fox-like African jackal, from
- the Gaboon, depress its ears when caressed. Wolves and jackals, when
- frightened, certainly tuck in their tails; and a tamed jackal has been
- described as careering round his master in circles and figures of
- eight, like a dog, with his tail between his legs.
- It has been stated[505] that foxes, however tame, never display any of
- the above expressive movements; but this is not strictly accurate. Many
- years ago I observed in the Zoological Gardens, and recorded the fact
- at the time, that a very tame English fox, When caressed by the keeper,
- wagged its tail, depressed its ears, and then threw itself on the
- ground, belly upwards. The black fox of North America likewise
- depressed its ears in a slight degree. But I believe that foxes never
- lick the hands of their masters, and I have been assured that when
- frightened they never tuck in their tails. If the explanation which I
- have given of the expression of affection in dogs be admitted, then it
- would appear that animals which have never been domesticated—namely
- wolves, jackals, and even foxes—have nevertheless acquired, through the
- principle of antithesis, certain expressive gestures; for it is not
- probable that these animals, confined in cages, should have learnt them
- by imitating dogs.
- _Cats_.—I have already described the actions of a cat (fig. 9), when
- feeling savage and not terrified. She assumes a crouching attitude and
- occasionally protrudes her fore-feet, with the claws exserted ready for
- striking. The tail is extended, being curled or lashed from side to
- side. The hair is not erected—at least it was not so in the few cases
- observed by me. The ears are drawn closely backwards and the teeth are
- shown. Low savage growls are uttered. We can understand why the
- attitude assumed by a cat when preparing to fight with another cat, or
- in any way greatly irritated, is so widely different from that of a dog
- approaching another dog with hostile intentions; for the cat uses her
- fore-feet for striking, and this renders a crouching position
- convenient or necessary. She is also much more accustomed than a dog to
- lie concealed and suddenly spring on her prey. No cause can be assigned
- with certainty for the tail being lashed or curled from side to side.
- This habit is common to many other animals—for instance, to the puma,
- when prepared to spring;[506] but it is not common to dogs, or to
- foxes, as I infer from Mr. St. John’s account of a fox lying in wait
- and seizing a hare. We have already seen that some kinds of lizards and
- various snakes, when excited, rapidly vibrate the tips of their tails.
- It would appear as if, under strong excitement, there existed an
- uncontrollable desire for movement of some kind, owing to nerve-force
- being freely liberated from the excited sensorium; and that as the tail
- is left free, and as its movement does not disturb the general position
- of the body, it is curled or lashed about.
- All the movements of a cat, when feeling affectionate, are in complete
- antithesis to those just described. She now stands upright, with
- slightly arched back, tail perpendicularly raised, and ears erected;
- and she rubs her cheeks and flanks against her master or mistress. The
- desire to rub something is so strong in cats under this state of mind,
- that they may often be seen rubbing themselves against the legs of
- chairs or tables, or against door-posts. This manner of expressing
- affection probably originated through association, as in the case of
- dogs, from the mother nursing and fondling her young; and perhaps from
- the young themselves loving each other and playing together. Another
- and very different gesture, expressive of pleasure, has already been
- described, namely, the curious manner in which young and even old cats,
- when pleased, alternately protrude their fore-feet, with separated
- toes, as if pushing against and sucking their mother’s teats. This
- habit is so far analogous to that of rubbing against something, that
- both apparently are derived from actions performed during the nursing
- period. Why cats should show affection by rubbing so much more than do
- dogs, though the latter delight in contact with their masters, and why
- cats only occasionally lick the hands of their friends, whilst dogs
- always do so, I cannot say. Cats cleanse themselves by licking their
- own coats more regularly than do dogs. On the other hand, their tongues
- seem less well fitted for the work than the longer and more flexible
- tongues of dogs.
- Cat Terrified at a Dog. Fig.15
- Cats, when terrified, stand at full height, and arch their backs in a
- well-known and ridiculous fashion. They spit, hiss, or growl. The hair
- over the whole body, and especially on the tail, becomes erect. In the
- instances observed by me the basal part of the tail was held upright,
- the terminal part being thrown on one side; but sometimes the tail (see
- fig. 15) is only a little raised, and is bent almost from the base to
- one side. The ears are drawn back, and the teeth exposed. When two
- kittens are playing together, the one often thus tries to frighten the
- other. From what we have seen in former chapters, all the above points
- of expression are intelligible, except the extreme arching of the back.
- I am inclined to believe that, in the same manner as many birds, whilst
- they ruffle their feathers, spread out their wings and tail, to make
- themselves look as big as possible, so cats stand upright at their full
- height, arch their backs, often raise the basal part of the tail, and
- erect their hair, for the same purpose. The lynx, when attacked, is
- said to arch its back, and is thus figured by Brehm. But the keepers in
- the Zoological Gardens have never seen any tendency to this action in
- the larger feline animals, such as tigers, lions, &c.; and these have
- little cause to be afraid of any other animal.
- Cats use their voices much as a means of expression, and they utter,
- under various emotions and desires, at least six or seven different
- sounds. The purr of satisfaction, which is made during both inspiration
- and expiration, is one of the most curious. The puma, cheetah, and
- ocelot likewise purr; but the tiger, when pleased, “emits a peculiar
- short snuffle, accompanied by the closure of the eyelids.”[507] It is
- said that the lion, jaguar, and leopard, do not purr.
- _Horses_.—Horses when savage draw their ears closely back, protrude
- their heads, and partially uncover their incisor teeth, ready for
- biting. When inclined to kick behind, they generally, through habit,
- draw back their ears; and their eyes are turned backwards in a peculiar
- manner.[508] When pleased, as when some coveted food is brought to them
- in the stable, they raise and draw in their heads, prick their ears,
- and looking intently towards their friend, often whinny. Impatience is
- expressed by pawing the ground.
- The actions of a horse when much startled are highly expressive. One
- day my horse was much frightened at a drilling machine, covered by a
- tarpaulin, and lying on an open field. He raised his head so high, that
- his neck became almost perpendicular; and this he did from habit, for
- the machine lay on a slope below, and could not have been seen with
- more distinctness through the raising of the head; nor if any sound had
- proceeded from it, could the sound have been more distinctly heard. His
- eyes and ears were directed intently forwards; and I could feel through
- the saddle the palpitations of his heart. With red dilated nostrils he
- snorted violently, and whirling round, would have dashed off at full
- speed, had I not prevented him. The distension of the nostrils is not
- for the sake of scenting the source of danger, for when a horse smells
- carefully at any object and is not alarmed, he does not dilate his
- nostrils. Owing to the presence of a valve in the throat, a horse when
- panting does not breathe through his open mouth, but through his
- nostrils; and these consequently have become endowed with great powers
- of expansion. This expansion of the nostrils, as well as the snorting,
- and the palpitations of the heart, are actions which have become firmly
- associated during a long series of generations with the emotion of
- terror; for terror has habitually led the horse to the most violent
- exertion in dashing away at full speed from the cause of danger.
- _Ruminants_.—Cattle and sheep are remarkable from displaying in so
- slight a degree their emotions or sensations, excepting that of extreme
- pain. A bull when enraged exhibits his rage only by the manner in which
- he holds his lowered head, with distended nostrils, and by bellowing.
- He also often paws the ground; but this pawing seems quite different
- from that of an impatient horse, for when the soil is loose, he throws
- up clouds of dust. I believe that bulls act in this manner when
- irritated by flies, for the sake of driving them away. The wilder
- breeds of sheep and the chamois when startled stamp on the ground, and
- whistle through their noses; and this serves as a danger-signal to
- their comrades. The musk-ox of the Arctic regions, when encountered,
- likewise stamps on the ground.[509] How this stamping action arose I
- cannot conjecture; for from inquiries which I have made it does not
- appear that any of these animals fight with their fore-legs.
- Some species of deer, when savage, display far more expression than do
- cattle, sheep, or goats, for, as has already been stated, they draw
- back their ears, grind their teeth, erect their hair, squeal, stamp on
- the ground, and brandish their horns. One day in the Zoological
- Gardens, the Formosan deer (_Cervus pseudaxis_) approached me in a
- curious attitude, with his muzzle raised high up, so that the horns
- were pressed back on his neck; the head being held rather obliquely.
- From the expression of his eye I felt sure that he was savage; he
- approached slowly, and as soon as he came close to the iron bars, he
- did not lower his head to butt at me, but suddenly bent it inwards, and
- struck his horns with great force against the railings. Mr. Bartlett
- informs me that some other species of deer place themselves in the same
- attitude when enraged.
- _Monkeys_.—The various species and genera of monkeys express their
- feelings in many different ways; and this fact is interesting, as in
- some degree bearing on the question, whether the so-called races of man
- should be ranked as distinct species or varieties; for, as we shall see
- in the following chapters, the different races of man express their
- emotions and sensations with remarkable uniformity throughout the
- world. Some of the expressive actions of monkeys are interesting in
- another way, namely from being closely analogous to those of man. As I
- have had no opportunity of observing any one species of the group under
- all circumstances, my miscellaneous remarks will be best arranged under
- different states of the mind.
- _Pleasure, joy, affection_—It is not possible to distinguish in
- monkeys, at least without more experience than I have had, the
- expression of pleasure or joy from that of affection. Young chimpanzees
- make a kind of barking noise, when pleased by the return of any one to
- whom they are attached. When this noise, which the keepers call a
- laugh, is uttered, the lips are protruded; but so they are under
- various other emotions. Nevertheless I could perceive that when they
- were pleased the form of the lips differed a little from that assumed
- when they were angered. If a young chimpanzee be tickled—and the
- armpits are particularly sensitive to tickling, as in the case of our
- children,—a more decided chuckling or laughing sound is uttered; though
- the laughter is sometimes noiseless. The corners of the mouth are then
- drawn backwards; and this sometimes causes the lower eyelids to be
- slightly wrinkled. But this wrinkling, which is so characteristic of
- our own laughter, is more plainly seen in some other monkeys. The teeth
- in the upper jaw in the chimpanzee are not exposed when they utter
- their laughing noise, in which respect they differ from us. But their
- eyes sparkle and grow brighter, as Mr. W. L. Martin,[510] who has
- particularly attended to their expression, states.
- Young Orangs, when tickled, likewise grin and make a chuckling sound;
- and Mr. Martin says that their eyes grow brighter. As soon as their
- laughter ceases, an expression may be detected passing over their
- faces, which, as Mr. Wallace remarked to me, may be called a smile. I
- have also noticed something of the same kind with the chimpanzee. Dr.
- Duchenne—and I cannot quote a better authority—informs me that he kept
- a very tame monkey in his house for a year; and when he gave it during
- meal-times some choice delicacy, he observed that the corners of its
- mouth were slightly raised; thus an expression of satisfaction,
- partaking of the nature of an incipient smile, and resembling that
- often seen on the face of main, could be plainly perceived in this
- animal.
- The _Cebus azaræ_,[511] when rejoiced at again seeing a beloved person,
- utters a peculiar tittering (_kichernden_) sound. It also expresses
- agreeable sensations, by drawing back the corners of its mouth, without
- producing any sound. Rengger calls this movement laughter, but it would
- be more appropriately called a smile. The form of the mouth is
- different when either pain or terror is expressed, and high shrieks are
- uttered. Another species of _Cebus_ in the Zoological Gardens (_C.
- hypoleucus_) when pleased, makes a reiterated shrill note, and likewise
- draws back the corners of its mouth, apparently through the contraction
- of the same muscles as with us. So does the Barbary ape (_Inuus
- ecaudatus_) to an extraordinary degree; and I observed in this monkey
- that the skin of the lower eyelids then became much wrinkled. At the
- same time it rapidly moved its lower jaw or lips in a spasmodic manner,
- the teeth being exposed; but the noise produced was hardly more
- distinct than that which we sometimes call silent laughter. Two of the
- keepers affirmed that this slight sound was the animal’s laughter, and
- when I expressed some doubt on this head (being at the time quite
- inexperienced), they made it attack or rather threaten a hated Entellus
- monkey, living in the same compartment. Instantly the whole expression
- of the face of the Inuus changed; the mouth was opened much more
- widely, the canine teeth were more fully exposed, and a hoarse barking
- noise was uttered.
- The Anubis baboon (_Cynocephalus anubis_) was first insulted and put
- into a furious rage, as was easily done, by his keeper, who then made
- friends with him and shook hands. As the reconciliation was effected
- the baboon rapidly moved up and down his jaws and lips, and looked
- pleased. When we laugh heartily, a similar movement, or quiver, may be
- observed more or less distinctly in our jaws; but with man the muscles
- of the chest are more particularly acted on, whilst with this baboon,
- and with some other monkeys, it is the muscles of the jaws and lips
- which are spasmodically affected.
- Cynopithecus Niger, in a Placid Condition. Fig.16-17
- I have already had occasion to remark on the curious manner in which
- two or three species of Alacacus and the _Cynopithecus niger_ draw back
- their ears and utter a slight jabbering noise, when they are pleased by
- being caressed. With the Cynopithecus (fig. 17), the corners of the
- mouth are at the same time drawn backwards and upwards, so that the
- teeth are exposed. Hence this expression would never be recognized by a
- stranger as one of pleasure. The crest of long hairs on the forehead is
- depressed, and apparently the whole skin of the head drawn backwards.
- The eyebrows are thus raised a little, and the eyes assume a staring
- appearance. The lower eyelids also become slightly wrinkled; but this
- wrinkling is not conspicuous, owing to the permanent transverse furrows
- on the face.
- _Painful emotions and sensations_.—With monkeys the expression of
- slight pain, or of any painful emotion, such as grief, vexation,
- jealousy, &c., is not easily distinguished from that of moderate anger;
- and these states of mind readily and quickly pass into each other.
- Grief, however, with some species is certainly exhibited by weeping. A
- woman, who sold a monkey to the Zoological Society, believed to have
- come from Borneo (_Macacus maurus_ or _M. inornatus_ of Gray), said
- that it often cried; and Mr. Bartlett, as well as the keeper Mr.
- Sutton, have repeatedly seen it, when grieved, or even when much
- pitied, weeping so copiously that the tears rolled down its cheeks.
- There is, however, something strange about this case, for two specimens
- subsequently kept in the Gardens, and believed to be the same species,
- have never been seen to weep, though they were carefully observed by
- the keeper and myself when much distressed and loudly screaming.
- Rengger states[512] that the eyes of the _Cebus azaræ_ fill with tears,
- but not sufficiently to overflow, when it is prevented getting some
- much desired object, or is much frightened. Humboldt also asserts that
- the eyes of the _Callithrix sciureus_ “instantly fill with tears when
- it is seized with fear;” but when this pretty little monkey in the
- Zoological Gardens was teased, so as to cry out loudly, this did not
- occur. I do not, however, wish to throw the least doubt on the accuracy
- of Humboldt’s statement.
- The appearance of dejection in young orangs and chimpanzees, when out
- of health, is as plain and almost as pathetic as in the case of our
- children. This state of mind and body is shown by their listless
- movements, fallen countenances, dull eyes, and changed complexion.
- _Anger_.—This emotion is often exhibited by many kinds of monkeys, and
- is expressed, as Mr. Martin remarks,[513] in many different ways. “Some
- species, when irritated, pout the lips, gaze with a fixed and savage
- glare on their foe, and make repeated short starts as if about to
- spring forward, uttering at the same time inward guttural sounds. Many
- display their anger by suddenly advancing, making abrupt starts, at the
- same time opening the mouth and pursing up the lips, so as to conceal
- the teeth, while the eyes are daringly fixed on the enemy, as if in
- savage defiance. Some again, and principally the long-tailed monkeys,
- or Guenons, display their teeth, and accompany their malicious grins
- with a sharp, abrupt, reiterated cry.” Mr. Sutton confirms the
- statement that some species uncover their teeth when enraged, whilst
- others conceal them by the protrusion of their lips; and some kinds
- draw back their ears. The _Cynopithecus niger_, lately referred to,
- acts in this manner, at the same time depressing the crest of hair on
- its forehead, and showing its teeth; so that the movements of the
- features from anger are nearly the same as those from pleasure; and the
- two expressions can be distinguished only by those familiar with the
- animal.
- Baboons often show their passion and threaten their enemies in a very
- odd manner, namely, by opening their mouths widely as in the act of
- yawning. Mr. Bartlett has often seen two baboons, when first placed in
- the same compartment, sitting opposite to each other and thus
- alternately opening their mouths; and this action seems frequently to
- end in a real yawn. Mr. Bartlett believes that both animals wish to
- show to each other that they are provided with a formidable set of
- teeth, as is undoubtedly the case. As I could hardly credit the reality
- of this yawning gesture, Mr. Bartlett insulted an old baboon and put
- him into a violent passion; and he almost immediately thus acted. Some
- species of Macacus and of Cereopithecus[514] behave in the same manner.
- Baboons likewise show their anger, as was observed by Brehin with those
- which he kept alive in Abyssinia, in another manner, namely, by
- striking the ground with one hand, “like an angry man striking the
- table with his fist.” I have seen this movement with the baboons in the
- Zoological Gardens; but sometimes the action seems rather to represent
- the searching for a stone or other object in their beds of straw.
- Mr. Sutton has often observed the face of the _Macacus rhesus_, when
- much enraged, growing red. As he was mentioning this to me, another
- monkey attacked a _rhesus_, and I saw its face redden as plainly as
- that of a man in a violent passion. In the course of a few minutes,
- after the battle, the face of this monkey recovered its natural tint.
- At the same time that the face reddened, the naked posterior part of
- the body, which is always red, seemed to grow still redder; but I
- cannot positively assert that this was the case. When the Mandrill is
- in any way excited, the brilliantly coloured, naked parts of the skin
- are said to become still more vividly coloured.
- With several species of baboons the ridge of the forehead projects much
- over the eyes, and is studded with a few long hairs, representing our
- eyebrows. These animals are always looking about them, and in order to
- look upwards they raise their eyebrows. They have thus, as it would
- appear, acquired the habit of frequently moving their eyebrows. However
- this may be, many kinds of monkeys, especially the baboons, when
- angered or in any way excited, rapidly and incessantly move their
- eyebrows up and down, as well as the hairy skin of their
- foreheads.[515] As we associate in the case of man the raising and
- lowering of the eyebrows with definite states of the mind, the almost
- incessant movement of the eyebrows by monkeys gives them a senseless
- expression. I once observed a man who had a trick of continually
- raising his eyebrows without any corresponding emotion, and this gave
- to him a foolish appearance; so it is with some persons who keep the
- corners of their mouths a little drawn backwards and upwards, as if by
- an incipient smile, though at the time they are not amused or pleased.
- A young orang, made jealous by her keeper attending to another monkey,
- slightly uncovered her teeth, and, uttering a peevish noise like
- _tish-shist_, turned her back on him. Both orangs and chimpanzees, when
- a little more angered, protrude their lips greatly, and make a harsh
- barking noise. A young female chimpanzee, in a violent passion,
- presented a curious resemblance to a child in the same state. She
- screamed loudly with widely open mouth, the lips being retracted so
- that the teeth were fully exposed. She threw her arms wildly about,
- sometimes clasping them over her head. She rolled on the ground,
- sometimes on her back, sometimes on her belly, and bit everything
- within reach. A young gibbon (_Hylobates syndactylus_) in a passion has
- been described[516] as behaving in almost exactly the same manner.
- The lips of young orangs and chimpanzees are protruded, sometimes to a
- wonderful degree, under various circumstances. They act thus, not only
- when slightly angered, sulky, or disappointed, but when alarmed at
- anything—in one instance, at the sight of a turtle,[517]—and likewise
- when pleased. But neither the degree of protrusion nor the shape of the
- mouth is exactly the same, as I believe, in all cases; and the sounds
- which are then uttered are different. The accompanying drawing
- represents a chimpanzee made sulky by an orange having been offered
- him, and then taken away. A similar protrusion or pouting of the lips,
- though to a much slighter degree, may be seen in sulky children.
- Chimpanzee Disappointed and Sulky. Fig. 18
- Many years ago, in the Zoological Gardens, I placed a looking-glass on
- the floor before two young orangs, who, as far as it was known, had
- never before seen one. At first they gazed at their own images with the
- most steady surprise, and often changed their point of view. They then
- approached close and protruded their lips towards the image, as if to
- kiss it, in exactly the same manner as they had previously done towards
- each other, when first placed, a few days before, in the same room.
- They next made all sorts of grimaces, and put themselves in various
- attitudes before the mirror; they pressed and rubbed the surface; they
- placed their hands at different distances behind it; looked behind it;
- and finally seemed almost frightened, started a little, became cross,
- and refused to look any longer.
- When we try to perform some little action which is difficult and
- requires precision, for instance, to thread a needle, we generally
- close our lips firmly, for the sake, I presume, of not disturbing our
- movements by breathing; and I noticed the same action in a young Orang.
- The poor little creature was sick, and was amusing itself by trying to
- kill the flies on the window-panes with its knuckles; this was
- difficult as the flies buzzed about, and at each attempt the lips were
- firmly compressed, and at the same time slightly protruded.
- Although the countenances, and more especially the gestures, of orangs
- and chimpanzees are in some respects highly expressive, I doubt whether
- on the whole they are so expressive as those of some other kinds of
- monkeys. This may be attributed in part to their ears being immovable,
- and in part to the nakedness of their eyebrows, of which the movements
- are thus rendered less conspicuous. When, however, they raise their
- eyebrows their foreheads become, as with us, transversely wrinkled. In
- comparison with man, their faces are inexpressive, chiefly owing to
- their not frowning under any emotion of the mind—that is, as far as I
- have been able to observe, and I carefully attended to this point.
- Frowning, which is one of the most important of all the expressions in
- man, is due to the contraction of the corrugators by which the eyebrows
- are lowered and brought together, so that vertical furrows are formed
- on the forehead. Both the orang and chimpanzee are said[518] to possess
- this muscle, but it seems rarely brought into action, at least in a
- conspicuous manner. I made my hands into a sort of cage, and placing
- some tempting fruit within, allowed both a young orang and chimpanzee
- to try their utmost to get it out; but although they grew rather cross,
- they showed not a trace of a frown. Nor was there any frown when they
- were enraged. Twice I took two chimpanzees from their rather dark room
- suddenly into bright sunshine, which would certainly have caused us to
- frown; they blinked and winked their eyes, but only once did I see a
- very slight frown. On another occasion, I tickled the nose of a
- chimpanzee with a straw, and as it crumpled up its face, slight
- vertical furrows appeared between the eyebrows. I have never seen a
- frown on the forehead of the orang.
- The gorilla, when enraged, is described as erecting its crest of hair,
- throwing down its under lip, dilating its nostrils, and uttering
- terrific yells. Messrs. Savage and Wyman[519] state that the scalp can
- be freely moved backwards and forwards, and that when the animal is
- excited it is strongly contracted; but I presume that they mean by this
- latter expression that the scalp is lowered; for they likewise speak of
- the young chimpanzee, when crying out, as having the eyebrows strongly
- contracted. The great power of movement in the scalp of the gorilla, of
- many baboons and other monkeys, deserves notice in relation to the
- power possessed by some few men, either through reversion or
- persistence, of voluntarily moving their scalps.[520]
- _Astonishment, Terror_—A living fresh-water turtle was placed at my
- request in the same compartment in the Zoological Gardens with many
- monkeys; and they showed unbounded astonishment, as well as some fear.
- This was displayed by their remaining motionless, staring intently with
- widely opened eyes, their eyebrows being often moved up and down. Their
- faces seemed somewhat lengthened. They occasionally raised themselves
- on their hind-legs to get abetter view. They often retreated a few
- feet, and then turning their heads over one shoulder, again stared
- intently. It was curious to observe how much less afraid they were of
- the turtle than of a living snake which I had formerly placed in their
- compartment;[521] for in the course of a few minutes some of the
- monkeys ventured to approach and touch the turtle. On the other hand,
- some of the larger baboons were greatly terrified, and grinned as if on
- the point of screaming out. When I showed a little dressed-up doll to
- the _Cynopithecus niger_, it stood motionless, stared intently with
- widely opened eyes, and advanced its ears a little forwards. But when
- the turtle was placed in its compartment, this monkey also moved its
- lips in an odd, rapid, jabbering manner, which the keeper declared was
- meant to conciliate or please the turtle.
- I was never able clearly to perceive that the eyebrows of astonished
- monkeys were kept permanently raised, though they were frequently moved
- up and down. Attention, which precedes astonishment, is expressed by
- man by a slight raising of the eyebrows; and Dr. Duchenne informs me
- that when he gave to the monkey formerly mentioned some quite new
- article of food, it elevated its eyebrows a little, thus assuming an
- appearance of close attention. It then took the food in its fingers,
- and, with lowered or rectilinear eyebrows, scratched, smelt, and
- examined it,—an expression of reflection being thus exhibited.
- Sometimes it would throw back its head a little, and again with
- suddenly raised eyebrows re-examine and finally taste the food.
- In no case did any monkey keep its mouth open when it was astonished.
- Mr. Sutton observed for me a young orang and chimpanzee during a
- considerable length of time; and however much they were astonished, or
- whilst listening intently to some strange sound, they did not keep
- their mouths open. This fact is surprising, as with mankind hardly any
- expression is more general than a widely open mouth under the sense of
- astonishment. As far as I have been able to observe, monkeys breathe
- more freely through their nostrils than men do; and this may account
- for their not opening their mouths when they are astonished; for, as we
- shall see in a future chapter, man apparently acts in this manner when
- startled, at first for the sake of quickly drawing a full inspiration,
- and afterwards for the sake of breathing as quietly as possible.
- Terror is expressed by many kinds of monkeys by the utterance of shrill
- screams; the lips being drawn back, so that the teeth are exposed. The
- hair becomes erect, especially when some anger is likewise felt. Mr.
- Sutton has distinctly seen the face of the _Macacus rhesus_ grow pale
- from fear. Monkeys also tremble from fear; and sometimes they void
- their excretions. I have seen one which, when caught, almost fainted
- from an excess of terror.
- Sufficient facts have now been given with respect to the expressions of
- various animals. It is impossible to agree with Sir C. Bell when he
- says[522] that “the faces of animals seem chiefly capable of expressing
- rage and fear;” and again, when he says that all their expressions “may
- be referred, more or less plainly, to their acts of volition or
- necessary instincts.” He who will look at a dog preparing to attack
- another dog or a man, and at the same animal when caressing his master,
- or will watch the countenance of a monkey when insulted, and when
- fondled by his keeper, will be forced to admit that the movements of
- their features and their gestures are almost as expressive as those of
- man. Although no explanation can be given of some of the expressions in
- the lower animals, the greater number are explicable in accordance with
- the three principles given at the commencement of the first chapter.
- CHAPTER VI. SPECIAL EXPRESSIONS OF MAN: SUFFERING AND WEEPING.
- The screaming and weeping of infants—Forms of features—Age at which
- weeping commences—The effects of habitual restraint on
- weeping—Sobbing—Cause of the contraction of the muscles round the eyes
- during screaming—Cause of the secretion of tears.
- In this and the following chapters the expressions exhibited by Man
- under various states of the mind will be described and explained, as
- far as lies in my power. My observations will be arranged according to
- the order which I have found the most convenient; and this will
- generally lead to opposite emotions and sensations succeeding each
- other.
- _Suffering of the body and mind: weeping_.—I have already described in
- sufficient detail, in the third chapter, the signs of extreme pain, as
- shown by screams or groans, with the writhing of the whole body and the
- teeth clenched or ground together. These signs are often accompanied or
- followed by profuse sweating, pallor, trembling, utter prostration, or
- faintness. No suffering is greater than that from extreme fear or
- horror, but here a distinct emotion comes into play, and will be
- elsewhere considered. Prolonged suffering, especially of the mind,
- passes into low spirits, grief, dejection, and despair, and these
- states will be the subject of the following chapter. Here I shall
- almost confine myself to weeping or crying, more especially in
- children.
- Infants, when suffering even slight pain, moderate hunger, or
- discomfort, utter violent and prolonged screams. Whilst thus screaming
- their eyes are firmly closed, so that the skin round them is wrinkled,
- and the forehead contracted into a frown. The mouth is widely opened
- with the lips retracted in a peculiar manner, which causes it to assume
- a squarish form; the gums or teeth being more or less exposed. The
- breath is inhaled almost spasmodically. It is easy to observe infants
- whilst screaming; but I have found photographs made by the
- instantaneous process the best means for observation, as allowing more
- deliberation. I have collected twelve, most of them made purposely for
- me; and they all exhibit the same general characteristics. I have,
- therefore, had six of them[601] (Plate I.) reproduced by the heliotype
- process.
- Screaming Infants. Plate I.
- The firm closing of the eyelids and consequent compression of the
- eyeball,—and this is a most important element in various
- expressions,—serves to protect the eyes from becoming too much gorged
- with blood, as will presently be explained in detail. With respect to
- the order in which the several muscles contract in firmly compressing
- the eyes, I am indebted to Dr. Langstaff, of Southampton, for some
- observations, which I have since repeated. The best plan for observing
- the order is to make a person first raise his eyebrows, and this
- produces transverse wrinkles across the forehead; and then very
- gradually to contract all the muscles round the elves with as much
- force as possible. The reader who is unacquainted with the anatomy of
- the face, ought to refer to p. 24, and look at the woodcuts 1 to 3. The
- corrugators of the brow (_corrugator supercilii_) seem to be the first
- muscles to contract; and these draw the eyebrows downwards and inwards
- towards the base of the nose, causing vertical furrows, that is a
- frown, to appear between the eyebrows; at the same time they cause the
- disappearance of the transverse wrinkles across the forehead. The
- orbicular muscles contract almost simultaneously with the corrugators,
- and produce wrinkles all round the eyes; they appear, however, to be
- enabled to contract with greater force, as soon as the contraction of
- the corrugators has given them some support. Lastly, the pyramidal
- muscles of the nose contract; and these draw the eyebrows and the skin
- of the forehead still lower down, producing short transverse wrinkles
- across the base of the nose.[602] For the sake of brevity these muscles
- will generally be spoken of as the orbiculars, or as those surrounding
- the eyes.
- When these muscles are strongly contracted, those running to the upper
- lip[603] likewise contract and raise the upper lip. This might have
- been expected from the manner in which at least one of them, the
- _malaris_, is connected with the orbiculars. Any one who will gradually
- contract the muscles round his eyes, will feel, as he increases the
- force, that his upper lip and the wings of his nose (which are partly
- acted on by one of the same muscles) are almost always a little drawn
- up. If he keeps his mouth firmly shut whilst contracting the muscles
- round the eyes, and then suddenly relaxes his lips, he will feel that
- the pressure on his eyes immediately increases. So again when a person
- on a bright, glaring day wishes to look at a distant object, but is
- compelled partially to close his eyelids, the upper lip may almost
- always be observed to be somewhat raised. The mouths of some very
- short-sighted persons, who are forced habitually to reduce the aperture
- of their eyes, wear from this same reason a grinning expression.
- The raising of the upper lip draws upwards the flesh of the upper parts
- of the cheeks, and produces a strongly marked fold on each cheek,—the
- naso-labial fold,—which runs from near the wings of the nostrils to the
- corners of the mouth and below them. This fold or furrow may be seen in
- all the photographs, and is very characteristic of the expression of a
- crying child; though a nearly similar fold is produced in the act of
- laughing or smiling.[604]
- As the upper lip is much drawn up during the act of screaming, in the
- manner just explained, the depressor muscles of the angles of the mouth
- (see K in woodcuts 1 and 2) are strongly contracted in order to keep
- the mouth widely open, so that a full volume of sound may be poured
- forth. The action of these opposed muscles, above and below, tends to
- give to the mouth an oblong, almost squarish outline, as may be seen in
- the accompanying photographs. An excellent observer,[605] in describing
- a baby crying whilst being fed, says, “it made its mouth like a square,
- and let the porridge run out at all four corners.” I believe, but we
- shall return to this point in a future chapter, that the depressor
- muscles of the angles of the mouth are less under the separate control
- of the will than the adjoining muscles; so that if a young child is
- only doubtfully inclined to cry, this muscle is generally the first to
- contract, and is the last to cease contracting. When older children
- commence crying, the muscles which run to the upper lip are often the
- first to contract; and this may perhaps be due to older children not
- having so strong a tendency to scream loudly, and consequently to keep
- their mouths widely open; so that the above-named depressor muscles are
- not brought into such strong action.
- With one of my own infants, from his eighth day and for some time
- afterwards, I often observed that the first sign of a screaming-fit,
- when it could be observed coming on gradually, was a little frown,
- owing to the contraction of the corrugators of the brows; the
- capillaries of the naked head and face becoming at the same time
- reddened with blood. As soon as the screaming-fit actually began, all
- the muscles round the eyes were strongly contracted, and the mouth
- widely opened in the manner above described; so that at this early
- period the features assumed the same form as at a more advanced age.
- Dr. Piderit[606] lays great stress on the contraction of certain
- muscles which draw down the nose and narrow the nostrils, as eminently
- characteristic of a crying expression. The _depressores anguli oris_,
- as we have just seen, are usually contracted at the same time, and they
- indirectly tend, according to Dr. Duchenne, to act in this same manner
- on the nose. With children having bad colds a similar pinched
- appearance of the nose may be noticed, which is at least partly due, as
- remarked to me by Dr. Langstaff, to their constant snuffling, and the
- consequent pressure of the atmosphere on the two sides. The purpose of
- this contraction of the nostrils by children having bad colds, or
- whilst crying, seems to be to check the downward flow of the mucus and
- tears, and to prevent these fluids spreading over the upper lip.
- After a prolonged and severe screaming-fit, the scalp, face, and eyes
- are reddened, owing to the return of the blood from the head having
- been impeded by the violent expiratory efforts; but the redness of the
- stimulated eyes is chiefly due to the copious effusion of tears. The
- various muscles of the face which have been strongly contracted, still
- twitch a little, and the upper lip is still slightly drawn up or
- everted,[607] with the corners of the mouth still a little drawn
- downwards. I have myself felt, and have observed in other grown-up
- persons, that when tears are restrained with difficulty, as in reading
- a pathetic story, it is almost impossible to prevent the various
- muscles. which with young children are brought into strong action
- during their screaming-fits, from slightly twitching or trembling.
- Infants whilst young do not shed tears or weep, as is well known to
- nurses and medical men. This circumstance is not exclusively due to the
- lacrymal glands being as yet incapable of secreting tears. I first
- noticed this fact from having accidentally brushed with the cuff of my
- coat the open eye of one of my infants, when seventy-seven days old,
- causing this eye to water freely; and though the child screamed
- violently, the other eye remained dry, or was only slightly suffused
- with tears. A similar slight effusion occurred ten days previously in
- both eyes during a screaming-fit. The tears did not run over the
- eyelids and roll down the cheeks of this child, whilst screaming badly,
- when 122 days old. This first happened 17 days later, at the age of 139
- days. A few other children have been observed for me, and the period of
- free weeping appears to be very variable. In one case, the eyes became
- slightly suffused at the age of only 20 days; in another, at 62 days.
- With two other children, the tears did NOT run down the face at the
- ages of 84 and 110 days; but in a third child they did run down at the
- age of 104 days. In one instance, as I was positively assured, tears
- ran down at the unusually early age of 42 days. It would appear as if
- the lacrymal glands required some practice in the individual before
- they are easily excited into action, in somewhat the same manner as
- various inherited consensual movements and tastes require some exercise
- before they are fixed and perfected. This is all the more likely with a
- habit like weeping, which must have been acquired since the period when
- man branched off from the common progenitor of the genus Homo and of
- the non-weeping anthropomorphous apes.
- The fact of tears not being shed at a very early age from pain or any
- mental emotion is remarkable, as, later in life, no expression is more
- general or more strongly marked than weeping. When the habit has once
- been acquired by an infant, it expresses in the clearest manner
- suffering of all kinds, both bodily pain and mental distress, even
- though accompanied by other emotions, such as fear or rage. The
- character of the crying, however, changes at a very early age, as I
- noticed in my own infants,—the passionate cry differing from that of
- grief. A lady informs me that her child, nine months old, when in a
- passion screams loudly, but does not weep; tears, however, are shed
- when she is punished by her chair being turned with its back to the
- table. This difference may perhaps be attributed to weeping being
- restrained, as we shall immediately see, at a more advanced age, under
- most circumstances excepting grief; and to the influence of such
- restraint being transmitted to an earlier period of life, than that at
- which it was first practised.
- With adults, especially of the male sex, weeping soon ceases to be
- caused by, or to express, bodily pain. This may be accounted for by its
- being thought weak and unmanly by men, both of civilized and barbarous
- races, to exhibit bodily pain by any outward sign. With this exception,
- savages weep copiously from very slight causes, of which fact Sir J.
- Lubbock[608] has collected instances. A New Zealand chief “cried like a
- child because the sailors spoilt his favourite cloak by powdering it
- with flour.” I saw in Tierra del Fuego a native who had lately lost a
- brother, and who alternately cried with hysterical violence, and
- laughed heartily at anything which amused him. With the civilized
- nations of Europe there is also much difference in the frequency of
- weeping. Englishmen rarely cry, except under the pressure of the
- acutest grief; whereas in some parts of the Continent the men shed
- tears much more readily and freely.
- The insane notoriously give way to all their emotions with little or no
- restraint; and I am informed by Dr. J. Crichton Browne, that nothing is
- more characteristic of simple melancholia, even in the male sex, than a
- tendency to weep on the slightest occasions, or from no cause. They
- also weep disproportionately on the occurrence of any real cause of
- grief. The length of time during which some patients weep is
- astonishing, as well as the amount of tears which they shed. One
- melancholic girl wept for a whole day, and afterwards confessed to Dr.
- Browne, that it was because she remembered that she had once shaved off
- her eyebrows to promote their growth. Many patients in the asylum sit
- for a long time rocking themselves backwards and forwards; “and if
- spoken to, they stop their movements, purse up their eyes, depress the
- corners of the mouth, and burst out crying.” In some of these cases,
- the being spoken to or kindly greeted appears to suggest some fanciful
- and sorrowful notion; but in other cases an effort of any kind excites
- weeping, independently of any sorrowful idea. Patients suffering from
- acute mania likewise have paroxysms of violent crying or blubbering, in
- the midst of their incoherent ravings. We must not, however, lay too
- much stress on the copious shedding of tears by the insane, as being
- due to the lack of all restraint; for certain brain-diseases, as
- hemiplegia, brain-wasting, and senile decay, have a special tendency to
- induce weeping. Weeping is common in the insane, even after a complete
- state of fatuity has been reached and the power of speech lost. Persons
- born idiotic likewise weep;[609] but it is said that this is not the
- case with cretins.
- Weeping seems to be the primary and natural expression, as we see in
- children, of suffering of any kind, whether bodily pain short of
- extreme agony, or mental distress. But the foregoing facts and common
- experience show us that a frequently repeated effort to restrain
- weeping, in association with certain states of the mind, does much in
- checking the habit. On the other hand, it appears that the power of
- weeping can be increased through habit; thus the Rev. R. Taylor,[610]
- who long resided in New Zealand, asserts that the women can voluntarily
- shed tears in abundance; they meet for this purpose to mourn for the
- dead, and they take pride in crying “in the most affecting manner.”
- A single effort of repression brought to bear on the lacrymal glands
- does little, and indeed seems often to lead to an opposite result. An
- old and experienced physician told me that he had always found that the
- only means to check the occasional bitter weeping of ladies who
- consulted him, and who themselves wished to desist, was earnestly to
- beg them not to try, and to assure them that nothing would relieve them
- so much as prolonged and copious crying.
- The screaming of infants consists of prolonged expirations, with short
- and rapid, almost spasmodic inspirations, followed at a somewhat more
- advanced age by sobbing. According to Gratiolet,[611] the glottis is
- chiefly affected during the act of sobbing. This sound is heard “at the
- moment when the inspiration conquers the resistance of the glottis, and
- the air rushes into the chest.” But the whole act of respiration is
- likewise spasmodic and violent. The shoulders are at the same time
- generally raised, as by this movement respiration is rendered easier.
- With one of my infants, when seventy-seven days old, the inspirations
- were so rapid and strong that they approached in character to sobbing;
- when 138 days old I first noticed distinct sobbing, which subsequently
- followed every bad crying-fit. The respiratory movements are partly
- voluntary and partly involuntary, and I apprehend that sobbing is at
- least in part due to children having some power to command after early
- infancy their vocal organs and to stop their screams, but from having
- less power over their respiratory muscles, these continue for a time to
- act in an involuntary or spasmodic manner, after having been brought
- into violent action. Sobbing seems to be peculiar to the human species;
- for the keepers in the Zoological Gardens assure me that they have
- never heard a sob from any kind of monkey; though monkeys often scream
- loudly whilst being chased and caught, and then pant for a long time.
- We thus see that there is a close analogy between sobbing and the free
- shedding of tears; for with children, sobbing does not commence during
- early infancy, but afterwards comes on rather suddenly and then follows
- every bad crying-fit, until the habit is checked with advancing years.
- _On the cause of the contraction of the muscles round the eyes during
- screaming_.—We have seen that infants and young children, whilst
- screaming, invariably close their eyes firmly, by the contraction of
- the surrounding muscles, so that the skin becomes wrinkled all around.
- With older children, and even with adults, whenever there is violent
- and unrestrained crying, a tendency to the contraction of these same
- muscles may be observed; though this is often checked in order not to
- interfere with vision.
- Sir C. Bell explains[612] this action in the following manner:—“During
- every violent act of expiration, whether in hearty laughter, weeping,
- coughing, or sneezing, the eyeball is firmly compressed by the fibres
- of the orbicularis; and this is a provision for supporting and
- defending the vascular system of the interior of the eye from a
- retrograde impulse communicated to the blood in the veins at that time.
- When we contract the chest and expel the air, there is a retardation of
- the blood in the veins of the neck and head; and in the more powerful
- acts of expulsion, the blood not only distends the vessels, but is even
- regurgitated into the minute branches. Were the eye not properly
- compressed at that time, and a resistance given to the shock,
- irreparable injury might be inflicted on the delicate textures of the
- interior of the eye.” He further adds, “If we separate the eyelids of a
- child to examine the eye, while it cries and struggles with passion, by
- taking off the natural support to the vascular system of the eye, and
- means of guarding it against the rush of blood then occurring, the
- conjunctiva becomes suddenly filled with blood, and the eyelids
- everted.”
- Not only are the muscles round the eyes strongly contracted, as Sir C.
- Bell states and as I have often observed, during screaming, loud
- laughter, coughing, and sneezing, but during several other analogous
- actions. A man contracts these muscles when he violently blows his
- nose. I asked one of my boys to shout as loudly as he possibly could,
- and as soon as he began, he firmly contracted his orbicular muscles; I
- observed this repeatedly, and on asking him why he had every time so
- firmly closed his eyes, I found that he was quite unaware of the fact:
- he had acted instinctively or unconsciously.
- It is not necessary, in order to lead to the contraction of these
- muscles, that air should actually be expelled from the chest; it
- suffices that the muscles of the chest and abdomen should contract with
- great force, whilst by the closure of the glottis no air escapes. In
- violent vomiting or retching the diaphragm is made to descend by the
- chest being filled with air; it is then held in this position by the
- closure of the glottis, “as well as by the contraction of its own
- fibres.”[613] The abdominal muscles now contract strongly upon the
- stomach, its proper muscles likewise contracting, and the contents are
- thus ejected. During each effort of vomiting “the head becomes greatly
- congested, so that the features are red and swollen, and the large
- veins of the face and temples visibly dilated.” At the same time, as I
- know from observation, the muscles round the eyes are strongly
- contracted. This is likewise the case when the abdominal muscles act
- downwards with unusual force in expelling the contents of the
- intestinal canal.
- The greatest exertion of the muscles of the body, if those of the chest
- are not brought into strong action in expelling or compressing the air
- within the lungs, does not lead to the contraction of the muscles round
- the eyes. I have observed my sons using great force in gymnastic
- exercises, as in repeatedly raising their suspended bodies by their
- arms alone, and in lifting heavy weights from the ground, but there was
- hardly any trace of contraction in the muscles round the eyes.
- As the contraction of these muscles for the protection of the eyes
- during violent expiration is indirectly, as we shall hereafter see, a
- fundamental element in several of our most important expressions, I was
- extremely anxious to ascertain how far Sir C. Bell’s view could be
- substantiated. Professor Donders, of Utrecht,[614] well known as one of
- the highest authorities in Europe on vision and on the structure of the
- eye, has most kindly undertaken for me this investigation with the aid
- of the many ingenious mechanisms of modern science, and has published
- the results.[615] He shows that during violent expiration the external,
- the intra-ocular, and the retro-ocular vessels of the eye are all
- affected in two ways, namely by the increased pressure of the blood in
- the arteries, and by the return of the blood in the veins being
- impeded. It is, therefore, certain that both the arteries and the veins
- of the eye are more or less distended during violent expiration. The
- evidence in detail may be found in Professor Donders’ valuable memoir.
- We see the effects on the veins of the head, in their prominence, and
- in the purple colour of the face of a man who coughs violently from
- being half choked. I may mention, on the same authority, that the whole
- eye certainly advances a little during each violent expiration. This is
- due to the dilatation of the retro-ocular vessels, and might have been
- expected from the intimate connection of the eye and brain; the brain
- being known to rise and fall with each respiration, when a portion of
- the skull has been removed; and as may be seen along the unclosed
- sutures of infants’ heads. This also, I presume, is the reason that the
- eyes of a strangled man appear as if they were starting from their
- sockets.
- With respect to the protection of the eye during violent expiratory
- efforts by the pressure of the eyelids, Professor Donders concludes
- from his various observations that this action certainly limits or
- entirely removes the dilatation of the vessels.[616] At such times, he
- adds, we not unfrequently see the hand involuntarily laid upon the
- eyelids, as if the better to support and defend the eyeball.
- Nevertheless much evidence cannot at present be advanced to prove that
- the eye actually suffers injury from the want of support during violent
- expiration; but there is some. It is “a fact that forcible expiratory
- efforts in violent coughing or vomiting, and especially in sneezing,
- sometimes give rise to ruptures of the little (external) vessels” of
- the eye.[617] With respect to the internal vessels, Dr. Gunning has
- lately recorded a case of exophthalmos in consequence of
- whooping-cough, which in his opinion depended on the rupture of the
- deeper vessels; and another analogous case has been recorded. But a
- mere sense of discomfort would probably suffice to lead to the
- associated habit of protecting the eyeball by the contraction of the
- surrounding muscles. Even the expectation or chance of injury would
- probably be sufficient, in the same manner as an object moving too near
- the eye induces involuntary winking of the eyelids. We may, therefore,
- safely conclude from Sir C. Bell’s observations, and more especially
- from the more careful investigations by Professor Donders, that the
- firm closure of the eyelids during the screaming of children is an
- action full of meaning and of real service.
- We have already seen that the contraction of the orbicular muscles
- leads to the drawing up of the upper lip, and consequently, if the
- mouth is kept widely open, to the drawing down of the corners by the
- contraction of the depressor muscles. The formation of the naso-labial
- fold on the cheeks likewise follows from the drawing up of the upper
- lip. Thus all the chief expressive movements of the face during crying
- apparently result from the contraction of the muscles round the eyes.
- We shall also find that the shedding of tears depends on, or at least
- stands in some connection with, the contraction of these same muscles.
- In some of the foregoing cases, especially in those of sneezing and
- coughing, it is possible that the contraction of the orbicular muscles
- may serve in addition to protect the eyes from too severe a jar or
- vibration. I think so, because dogs and cats, in crunching hard bones,
- always close their eyelids, and at least sometimes in sneezing; though
- dogs do not do so whilst barking loudly. Mr. Sutton carefully observed
- for me a young orang and chimpanzee, and he found that both always
- closed their eyes in sneezing and coughing, but not whilst screaming
- violently. I gave a small pinch of snuff to a monkey of the American
- division, namely, a Cebus, and it closed its eyelids whilst sneezing;
- but not on a subsequent occasion whilst uttering loud cries.
- _Cause of the secretion of tears_.—It is an important fact which must
- be considered in any theory of the secretion of tears from the mind
- being affected, that whenever the muscles round the eyes are strongly
- and involuntarily contracted in order to compress the blood-vessels and
- thus to protect the eyes, tears are secreted, often in sufficient
- abundance to roll down the cheeks. This occurs under the most opposite
- emotions, and under no emotion at all. The sole exception, and this is
- only a partial one, to the existence of a relation between the
- involuntary and strong contraction of these muscles and the secretion
- of tears is that of young infants, who, whilst screaming violently with
- their eyelids firmly closed, do not commonly weep until they have
- attained the age of from two to three or four months. Their eyes,
- however, become suffused with tears at a much earlier age. It would
- appear, as already remarked, that the lacrymal glands do not, from the
- want of practice or some other cause, come to full functional activity
- at a very early period of life. With children at a somewhat later age,
- crying out or wailing from any distress is so regularly accompanied by
- the shedding of tears, that weeping and crying are synonymous
- terms.[618]
- Under the opposite emotion of great joy or amusement, as long as
- laughter is moderate there is hardly any contraction of the muscles
- round the eyes, so that there is no frowning; but when peals of loud
- laughter are uttered, with rapid and violent spasmodic expirations,
- tears stream down the face. I have more than once noticed the face of a
- person, after a paroxysm of violent laughter, and I could see that the
- orbicular muscles and those running to the upper lip were still
- partially contracted, which together with the tear-stained cheeks gave
- to the upper half of the face an expression not to be distinguished
- from that of a child still blubbering from grief. The fact of tears
- streaming down the face during violent laughter is common to all the
- races of mankind, as we shall see in a future chapter.
- In violent coughing especially when a person is half-choked, the face
- becomes purple, the veins distended, the orbicular muscles strongly
- contracted, and tears run down the cheeks. Even after a fit of ordinary
- coughing, almost every one has to wipe his eyes. In violent vomiting or
- retching, as I have myself experienced and seen in others, the
- orbicular muscles are strongly contracted, and tears sometimes flow
- freely down the cheeks. It has been suggested to me that this may be
- due to irritating matter being injected into the nostrils, and causing
- by reflex action the secretion of tears. Accordingly I asked one of my
- informants, a surgeon, to attend to the effects of retching when
- nothing was thrown up from the stomach; and, by an odd coincidence, he
- himself suffered the next morning from an attack of retching, and three
- days subsequently observed a lady under a similar attack; and he is
- certain that in neither case an atom of matter was ejected from the
- stomach; yet the orbicular muscles were strongly contracted, and tears
- freely secreted. I can also speak positively to the energetic
- contraction of these same muscles round the eyes, and to the coincident
- free secretion of tears, when the abdominal muscles act with unusual
- force in a downward direction on the intestinal canal.
- Yawning commences with a deep inspiration, followed by a long and
- forcible expiration; and at the same time almost all the muscles of the
- body are strongly contracted, including those round the eyes. During
- this act tears are often secreted, and I have seen them even rolling
- down the cheeks.
- I have frequently observed that when persons scratch some point which
- itches intolerably, they forcibly close their eyelids; but they do not,
- as I believe, first draw a deep breath and then expel it with force;
- and I have never noticed that the eyes then become filled with tears;
- but I am not prepared to assert that this does not occur. The forcible
- closure of the eyelids is, perhaps, merely a part of that general
- action by which almost all the muscles of the body are at the same time
- rendered rigid. It is quite different from the gentle closure of the
- eyes which often accompanies, as Gratiolet remarks,[619] the smelling a
- delicious odour, or the tasting a delicious morsel, and which probably
- originates in the desire to shut out any disturbing impression through
- the eyes.
- Professor Donders writes to me to the following effect: “I have
- observed some cases of a very curious affection when, after a slight
- rub (_attouchement_), for example, from the friction of a coat, which
- caused neither a wound nor a contusion, spasms of the orbicular muscles
- occurred, with a very profuse flow of tears, lasting about one hour.
- Subsequently, sometimes after an interval of several weeks, violent
- spasms of the same muscles re-occurred, accompanied by the secretion of
- tears, together with primary or secondary redness of the eye.” Mr.
- Bowman informs me that he has occasionally observed closely analogous
- cases, and that, in some of these, there was no redness or inflammation
- of the eyes.
- I was anxious to ascertain whether there existed in any of the lower
- animals a similar relation between the contraction of the orbicular
- muscles during violent expiration and the secretion of tears; but there
- are very few animals which contract these muscles in a prolonged
- manner, or which shed tears. _The Macacus maurus_, which formerly wept
- so copiously in the Zoological Gardens, would have been a fine case for
- observation; but the two monkeys now there, and which are believed to
- belong to the same species, do not weep. Nevertheless they were
- carefully observed by Mr. Bartlett and myself, whilst screaming loudly,
- and they seemed to contract these muscles; but they moved about their
- cages so rapidly, that it was difficult to observe with certainty. No
- other monkey, as far as I have been able to ascertain, contracts its
- orbicular muscles whilst screaming.
- The Indian elephant is known sometimes to weep. Sir E. Tennent, in
- describing these which he saw captured and bound in Ceylon, says, some
- “lay motionless on the ground, with no other indication of suffering
- than the tears which suffused their eyes and flowed incessantly.”
- Speaking of another elephant he says, “When overpowered and made fast,
- his grief was most affecting; his violence sank to utter prostration,
- and he lay on the ground, uttering choking cries, with tears trickling
- down his cheeks.”[620] In the Zoological Gardens the keeper of the
- Indian elephants positively asserts that he has several times seen
- tears rolling down the face of the old female, when distressed by the
- removal of the young one. Hence I was extremely anxious to ascertain,
- as an extension of the relation between the contraction of the
- orbicular muscles and the shedding of tears in man, whether elephants
- when screaming or trumpeting loudly contract these muscles. At Mr.
- Bartlett’s desire the keeper ordered the old and the young elephant to
- trumpet; and we repeatedly saw in both animals that, just as the
- trumpeting began, the orbicular muscles, especially the lower ones,
- were distinctly contracted. On a subsequent occasion the keeper made
- the old elephant trumpet much more loudly, and invariably both the
- upper and lower orbicular muscles were strongly contracted, and now in
- an equal degree. It is a singular fact that the African elephant,
- which, however, is so different from the Indian species that it is
- placed by some naturalists in a distinct sub-genus, when made on two
- occasions to trumpet loudly, exhibited no trace of the contraction of
- the orbicular muscles.
- From the several foregoing cases with respect to Man, there can, I
- think, be no doubt that the contraction of the muscles round the eyes,
- during violent expiration or when the expanded chest is forcibly
- compressed, is, in some manner, intimately connected with the secretion
- of tears. This holds good under widely different emotions, and
- independently of any emotion. It is not, of course, meant that tears
- cannot be secreted without the contraction of these muscles; for it is
- notorious that they are often freely shed with the eyelids not closed,
- and with the brows unwrinkled. The contraction must be both involuntary
- and prolonged, as during a choking fit, or energetic, as during a
- sneeze. The mere involuntary winking of the eyelids, though often
- repeated, does not bring tears into the eyes. Nor does the voluntary
- and prolonged contraction of the several surrounding muscles suffice.
- As the lacrymal glands of children are easily excited, I persuaded my
- own and several other children of different ages to contract these
- muscles repeatedly with their utmost force, and to continue doing so as
- long as they possibly could; but this produced hardly any effect. There
- was sometimes a little moisture in the eyes, but not more than
- apparently could be accounted for by the squeezing out of the already
- secreted tears within the glands.
- The nature of the relation between the involuntary and energetic
- contraction of the muscles round the eyes, and the secretion of tears,
- cannot be positively ascertained, but a probable view may be suggested.
- The primary function of the secretion of tears, together with some
- mucus, is to lubricate the surface of the eye; and a secondary one, as
- some believe, is to keep the nostrils damp, so that the inhaled air may
- be moist,[621] and likewise to favour the power of smelling. But
- another, and at least equally important function of tears, is to wash
- out particles of dust or other minute objects which may get into the
- eyes. That this is of great importance is clear from the cases in which
- the cornea has been rendered opaque through inflammation, caused by
- particles of dust not being removed, in consequence of the eye and
- eyelid becoming immovable.[622] The secretion of tears from the
- irritation of any foreign body in the eye is a reflex action;—that is,
- the body irritates a peripheral nerve which sends an impression to
- certain sensory nerve-cells; these transmit an influence to other
- cells, and these again to the lacrymal glands. The influence
- transmitted to these glands causes, as there is good reason to believe,
- the relaxation of the muscular coats of the smaller arteries; this
- allows more blood to permeate the glandular tissue, and this induces a
- free secretion of tears. When the small arteries of the face, including
- those of the retina, are relaxed under very different circumstances,
- namely, during an intense blush, the lacrymal glands are sometimes
- affected in a like manner, for the eyes become suffused with tears.
- It is difficult to conjecture how many reflex actions have originated,
- but, in relation to the present case of the affection of the lacrymal
- glands through irritation of the surface of the eye, it may be worth
- remarking that, as soon as some primordial form became semi-terrestrial
- in its habits, and was liable to get particles of dust into its eyes,
- if these were not washed out they would cause much irritation; and on
- the principle of the radiation of nerve-force to adjoining nerve-cells,
- the lacrymal glands would be stimulated to secretion. As this would
- often recur, and as nerve-force readily passes along accustomed
- channels, a slight irritation would ultimately suffice to cause a free
- secretion of tears.
- As soon as by this, or by some other means, a reflex action of this
- nature had been established and rendered easy, other stimulants applied
- to the surface of the eye—such as a cold wind, slow inflammatory
- action, or a blow on the eyelids—would cause a copious secretion of
- tears, as we know to be the case. The glands are also excited into
- action through the irritation of adjoining parts. Thus when the
- nostrils are irritated by pungent vapours, though the eyelids may be
- kept firmly closed, tears are copiously secreted; and this likewise
- follows from a blow on the nose, for instance from a boxing-glove. A
- stinging switch on the face produces, as I have seen, the same effect.
- In these latter cases the secretion of tears is an incidental result,
- and of no direct service. As all these parts of the face, including the
- lacrymal glands, are supplied with branches of the same nerve, namely,
- the fifth, it is in some degree intelligible that the effects of the
- excitement of any one branch should spread to the nerve-cells or roots
- of the other branches.
- The internal parts of the eye likewise act, under certain conditions,
- in a reflex manner on the lacrymal glands. The following statements
- have been kindly communicated to me by Mr. Bowman; but the subject is a
- very intricate one, as all the parts of the eye are so intimately
- related together, and are so sensitive to various stimulants. A strong
- light acting on the retina, when in a normal condition, has very little
- tendency to cause lacrymation; but with unhealthy children having
- small, old-standing ulcers on the cornea, the retina becomes
- excessively sensitive to light, and exposure even to common daylight
- causes forcible and sustained closure of the lids, and a profuse flow
- of tears. When persons who ought to begin the use of convex glasses
- habitually strain the waning power of accommodation, an undue secretion
- of tears very often follows, and the retina is liable to become unduly
- sensitive to light. In general, morbid affections of the surface of the
- eye, and of the ciliary structures concerned in the accommodative act,
- are prone to be accompanied with excessive secretion of tears. Hardness
- of the eyeball, not rising to inflammation, but implying a want of
- balance between the fluids poured out and again taken up by the
- intra-ocular vessels, is not usually attended with any lacrymation.
- When the balance is on the other side, and the eye becomes too soft,
- there is a greater tendency to lacrymation. Finally, there are numerous
- morbid states and structural alterations of the eyes, and even terrible
- inflammations, which may be attended with little or no secretion of
- tears.
- It also deserves notice, as indirectly bearing on our subject, that the
- eye and adjoining parts are subject to an extraordinary number of
- reflex and associated movements, sensations, and actions, besides those
- relating to the lacrymal glands. When a bright light strikes the retina
- of one eye alone, the iris contracts, but the iris of the other eye
- moves after a measurable interval of time. The iris likewise moves in
- accommodation to near or distant vision, and when the two eyes are made
- to converge.[623] Every one knows how irresistibly the eyebrows are
- drawn down under an intensely bright light. The eyelids also
- involuntarily wink when an object is moved near the eyes, or a sound is
- suddenly heard. The well-known case of a bright light causing some
- persons to sneeze is even more curious; for nerve-force here radiates
- from certain nerve-cells in connection with the retina, to the sensory
- nerve-cells of the nose, causing it to tickle; and from these, to the
- cells which command the various respiratory muscles (the orbiculars
- included) which expel the air in so peculiar a manner that it rushes
- through the nostrils alone.
- To return to our point: why are tears secreted during a screaming-fit
- or other violent expiratory efforts? As a slight blow on the eyelids
- causes a copious secretion of tears, it is at least possible that the
- spasmodic contraction of the eyelids, by pressing strongly on the
- eyeball, should in a similar manner cause some secretion. This seems
- possible, although the voluntary contraction of the same muscles does
- not produce any such effect. We know that a man cannot voluntarily
- sneeze or cough with nearly the same force as he does automatically;
- and so it is with the contraction of the orbicular muscles: Sir C. Bell
- experimented on them, and found that by suddenly and forcibly closing
- the eyelids in the dark, sparks of light are seen, like those caused by
- tapping the eyelids with the fingers; “but in sneezing the compression
- is both more rapid and more forcible, and the sparks are more
- brilliant.” That these sparks are due to the contraction of the eyelids
- is clear, because if they “are held open during the act of sneezing, no
- sensation of light will be experienced.” In the peculiar cases referred
- to by Professor Donders and Mr. Bowman, we have seen that some weeks
- after the eye has been very slightly injured, spasmodic contractions of
- the eyelids ensue, and these are accompanied by a profuse flow of
- tears. In the act of yawning, the tears are apparently due solely to
- the spasmodic contraction of the muscles round the eyes.
- Notwithstanding these latter cases, it seems hardly credible that the
- pressure of the eyelids on the surface of the eye, although effected
- spasmodically and therefore with much greater force than can be done
- voluntarily, should be sufficient to cause by reflex action the
- secretion of tears in the many cases in which this occurs during
- violent expiratory efforts.
- Another cause may come conjointly into play. We have seen that the
- internal parts of the eye, under certain conditions act in a reflex
- manner on the lacrymal glands. We know that during violent expiratory
- efforts the pressure of the arterial blood within the vessels of the
- eye is increased, and that the return of the venous blood is impeded.
- It seems, therefore, not improbable that the distension of the ocular
- vessels, thus induced, might act by reflection on the lacrymal
- glands—the effects due to the spasmodic pressure of the eyelids on the
- surface of the eye being thus increased.
- In considering how far this view is probable, we should bear in mind
- that the eyes of infants have been acted on in this double manner
- during numberless generations, whenever they have screamed; and on the
- principle of nerve-force readily passing along accustomed channels,
- even a moderate compression of the eyeballs and a moderate distension
- of the ocular vessels would ultimately come, through habit, to act on
- the glands. We have an analogous case in the orbicular muscles being
- almost always contracted in some slight degree, even during a gentle
- crying-fit, when there can be no distension of the vessels and no
- uncomfortable sensation excited within the eyes.
- Moreover, when complex actions or movements have long been performed in
- strict association together, and these are from any cause at first
- voluntarily and afterwards habitually checked, then if the proper
- exciting conditions occur, any part of the action or movement which is
- least under the control of the will, will often still be involuntarily
- performed. The secretion by a gland is remarkably free from the
- influence of the will; therefore, when with the advancing age of the
- individual, or with the advancing culture of the race, the habit of
- crying out or screaming is restrained, and there is consequently no
- distension of the blood-vessels of the eye, it may nevertheless well
- happen that tears should still be secreted. We may see, as lately
- remarked, the muscles round the eyes of a person who reads a pathetic
- story, twitching or trembling in so slight a degree as hardly to be
- detected. In this case there has been no screaming and no distension of
- the blood-vessels, yet through habit certain nerve-cells send a small
- amount of nerve-force to the cells commanding the muscles round the
- eyes; and they likewise send some to the cells commanding the lacrymal
- glands, for the eyes often become at the same time just moistened with
- tears. If the twitching of the muscles round the eyes and the secretion
- of tears had been completely prevented, nevertheless it is almost
- certain that there would have been some tendency to transmit
- nerve-force in these same directions; and as the lacrymal glands are
- remarkably free from the control of the will, they would be eminently
- liable still to act, thus betraying, though there were no other outward
- signs, the pathetic thoughts which were passing through the person’s
- mind.
- As a further illustration of the view here advanced, I may remark that
- if, during an early period of life, when habits of all kinds are
- readily established, our infants, when pleased, had been accustomed to
- utter loud peals of laughter (during which the vessels of their eyes
- are distended) as often and as continuously as they have yielded when
- distressed to screaming-fits, then it is probable that in after life
- tears would have been as copiously and as regularly secreted under the
- one state of mind as under the other. Gentle laughter, or a smile, or
- even a pleasing thought, would have sufficed to cause a moderate
- secretion of tears. There does indeed exist an evident tendency in this
- direction, as will be seen in a future chapter, when we treat of the
- tender feelings. With the Sandwich Islanders, according to
- Freycinet,[624] tears are actually recognized as a sign of happiness;
- but we should require better evidence on this head than that of a
- passing voyager. So again if our infants, during many generations, and
- each of them during several years, had almost daily suffered from
- prolonged choking-fits, during which the vessels of the eye are
- distended and tears copiously secreted, then it is probable, such is
- the force of associated habit, that during after life the mere thought
- of a choke, without any distress of mind, would have sufficed to bring
- tears into our eyes.
- To sum up this chapter, weeping is probably the result of some such
- chain of events as follows. Children, when wanting food or suffering in
- any way, cry out loudly, like the young of most other animals, partly
- as a call to their parents for aid, and partly from any great exertion
- serving relief. Prolonged screaming inevitably leads to the gorging of
- the blood-vessels of the eye; and this will have led, at first
- consciously and at last habitually, to the contraction of the muscles
- round the eyes in order to protect them. At the same time the spasmodic
- pressure on the surface of the eye, and the distension of the vessels
- within the eye, without necessarily entailing any conscious sensation,
- will have affected, through reflex action, the lacrymal glands.
- Finally, through the three principles of nerve-force readily passing
- along accustomed channels—of association, which is so widely extended
- in its power—and of certain actions, being more under the control of
- the will than others—it has come to pass that suffering readily causes
- the secretion of tears, without being necessarily accompanied by any
- other action.
- Although in accordance with this view we must look at weeping as an
- incidental result, as purposeless as the secretion of tears from a blow
- outside the eye, or as a sneeze from the retina being affected by a
- bright light, yet this does not present any difficulty in our
- understanding how the secretion of tears serves as a relief to
- suffering. And by as much as the weeping is more violent or hysterical,
- by so much will the relief be greater,—on the same principle that the
- writhing of the whole body, the grinding of the teeth, and the uttering
- of piercing shrieks, all give relief under an agony of pain.
- CHAPTER VII. LOW SPIRITS, ANXIETY, GRIEF, DEJECTION, DESPAIR.
- General effect of grief on the system—Obliquity of the eyebrows under
- suffering—On the cause of the obliquity of the eyebrows—On the
- depression of the corners of the mouth.
- After the mind has suffered from an acute paroxysm of grief, and the
- cause still continues, we fall into a state of low spirits; or we may
- be utterly cast down and dejected. Prolonged bodily pain, if not
- amounting to an agony, generally leads to the same state of mind. If we
- expect to suffer, we are anxious; if we have no hope of relief, we
- despair.
- Persons suffering from excessive grief often seek relief by violent and
- almost frantic movements, as described in a former chapter; but when
- their suffering is somewhat mitigated, yet prolonged, they no longer
- wish for action, but remain motionless and passive, or may occasionally
- rock themselves to and fro. The circulation becomes languid; the face
- pale; the muscles flaccid; the eyelids droop; the head hangs on the
- contracted chest; the lips, cheeks, and lower jaw all sink downwards
- from their own weight. Hence all the features are lengthened; and the
- face of a person who hears bad news is said to fall. A party of natives
- in Tierra del Fuego endeavoured to explain to us that their friend, the
- captain of a sealing vessel, was out of spirits, by pulling down their
- cheeks with both hands, so as to make their faces as long as possible.
- Mr. Bunnet informs me that the Australian aborigines when out of
- spirits have a chop-fallen appearance. After prolonged suffering the
- eyes become dull and lack expression, and are often slightly suffused
- with tears. The eyebrows not rarely are rendered oblique, which is due
- to their inner ends being raised. This produces peculiarly-formed
- wrinkles on the forehead, which are very different from those of a
- simple frown; though in some cases a frown alone may be present. The
- comers of the mouth are drawn downwards, which is so universally
- recognized as a sign of being out of spirits, that it is almost
- proverbial.
- The breathing becomes slow and feeble, and is often interrupted by deep
- sighs. As Gratiolet remarks, whenever our attention is long
- concentrated on any subject, we forget to breathe, and then relieve
- ourselves by a deep inspiration; but the sighs of a sorrowful person,
- owing to his slow respiration and languid circulation, are eminently
- characteristic.[701] As the grief of a person in this state
- occasionally recurs and increases into a paroxysm, spasms affect the
- respiratory muscles, and he feels as if something, the so-called
- _globus hystericus_, was rising in his throat. These spasmodic
- movements are clearly allied to the sobbing of children, and are
- remnants of those severer spasms which occur when a person is said to
- choke from excessive grief.[702]
- _Obliquity of the eyebrows_.—Two points alone in the above description
- require further elucidation, and these are very curious ones; namely,
- the raising of the inner ends of the eyebrows, and the drawing down of
- the corners of the mouth. With respect to the eyebrows, they may
- occasionally be seen to assume an oblique position in persons suffering
- from deep dejection or anxiety; for instance, I have observed this
- movement in a mother whilst speaking about her sick son; and it is
- sometimes excited by quite trifling or momentary causes of real or
- pretended distress. The eyebrows assume this position owing to the
- contraction of certain muscles (namely, the orbiculars, corrugators,
- and pyramidals of the nose, which together tend to lower and contract
- the eyebrows) being partially cheeked by the more powerful action of
- the central fasciæ of the frontal muscle. These latter fasciæ by their
- contraction raise the inner ends alone of the eyebrows; and as the
- corrugators at the same time draw the eyebrows together, their inner
- ends become puckered into a fold or lump. This fold is a highly
- characteristic point in the appearance of the eyebrows when rendered
- oblique, as may be seen in figs. 2 and 5, Plate II. The eyebrows are at
- the same time somewhat roughened, owing to the hairs being made to
- project. Dr. J. Crichton Browne has also often noticed in melancholic
- patients who keep their eyebrows persistently oblique, “a peculiar
- acute arching of the upper eyelid.” A trace of this may be observed by
- comparing the right and left eyelids of the young man in the photograph
- (fig. 2, Plate II.); for he was not able to act equally on both
- eyebrows. This is also shown by the unequal furrows on the two sides of
- his forehead. The acute arching of the eyelids depends, I believe, on
- the inner end alone of the eyebrows being raised; for when the whole
- eyebrow is elevated and arched, the upper eyelid follows in a slight
- degree the same movement.
- Obliquity of the Eyebrows. Plate II
- But the most conspicuous result of the opposed contraction of the
- above-named muscles, is exhibited by the peculiar furrows formed on the
- forehead. These muscles, when thus in conjoint yet opposed action, may
- be called, for the sake of brevity, the grief-muscles. When a person
- elevates his eyebrows by the contraction of the whole frontal muscle,
- transverse wrinkles extend across the whole breadth of the forehead;
- but in the present case the middle fasciae alone are contracted;
- consequently, transverse furrows are formed across the middle part
- alone of the forehead. The skin over the exterior parts of both
- eyebrows is at the same time drawn downwards and smooth, by the
- contraction of the outer portions of the orbicular muscles. The
- eyebrows are likewise brought together through the simultaneous
- contraction of the corrugators;[703] and this latter action generates
- vertical furrows, separating the exterior and lowered part of the skin
- of the forehead from the central and raised part. The union of these
- vertical furrows with the central and transverse furrows (see figs. 2
- and 3) produces a mark on the forehead which has been compared to a
- horse-shoe; but the furrows more strictly form three sides of a
- quadrangle. They are often conspicuous on the foreheads of adult or
- nearly adult persons, when their eyebrows are made oblique; but with
- young children, owing to their skin not easily wrinkling, they are
- rarely seen, or mere traces of them can be detected.
- These peculiar furrows are best represented in fig. 3, Plate II., on
- the forehead of a young lady who has the power in an unusual degree of
- voluntarily acting on the requisite muscles. As she was absorbed in the
- attempt, whilst being photographed, her expression was not at all one
- of grief; I have therefore given the forehead alone. Fig. 1 on the same
- plate, copied from Dr. Duchenne’s work,[704] represents, on a reduced
- scale, the face, in its natural state, of a young man who was a good
- actor. In fig. 2 he is shown simulating grief, but the two eyebrows, as
- before remarked, are not equally acted on. That the expression is true,
- may be inferred from the fact that out of fifteen persons, to whom the
- original photograph was shown, without any clue to what was intended
- being given them, fourteen immediately answered, “despairing sorrow,”
- “suffering endurance,” “melancholy,” and so forth. The history of fig.
- 5 is rather curious: I saw the photograph in a shop-window, and took it
- to Mr. Rejlander for the sake of finding out by whom it had been made;
- remarking to him how pathetic the expression was. He answered, “I made
- it, and it was likely to be pathetic, for the boy in a few minutes
- burst out crying.” He then showed me a photograph of the same boy in a
- placid state, which I have had (fig. 4) reproduced. In fig. 6, a trace
- of obliquity in the eyebrows may be detected; but this figure, as well
- as fig. 7, is given to show the depression of the corners of the mouth,
- to which subject I shall presently refer.
- Few persons, without some practice, can voluntarily act on their
- grief-muscles; but after repeated trials a considerable number succeed,
- whilst others never can. The degree of obliquity in the eyebrows,
- whether assumed voluntarily or unconsciously, differs much in different
- persons. With some who apparently have unusually strong pyramidal
- muscles, the contraction of the central fasciae of the frontal muscle,
- although it may be energetic, as shown by the quadrangular furrows on
- the forehead, does not raise the inner ends of the eyebrows, but only
- prevents their being so much lowered as they otherwise would have been.
- As far as I have been able to observe, the grief-muscles are brought
- into action much more frequently by children and women than by men.
- They are rarely acted on, at least with grown-up persons, from bodily
- pain, but almost exclusively from mental distress. Two persons who,
- after some practice, succeeded in acting on their grief-muscles, found
- by looking at a mirror that when they made their eyebrows oblique, they
- unintentionally at the same time depressed the corners of their mouths;
- and this is often the case when the expression is naturally assumed.
- The power to bring the grief-muscles freely into play appears to be
- hereditary, like almost every other human faculty. A lady belonging to
- a family famous for having produced an extraordinary number of great
- actors and actresses, and who can herself give this expression “with
- singular precision,” told Dr. Crichton Browne that all her family had
- possessed the power in a remarkable degree. The same hereditary
- tendency is said to have extended, as I likewise hear from Dr. Browne,
- to the last descendant of the family, which gave rise to Sir Walter
- Scott’s novel of ‘Red Gauntlet;’ but the hero is described as
- contracting his forehead into a horseshoe mark from any strong emotion.
- I have also seen a young woman whose forehead seemed almost habitually
- thus contracted, independently of any emotion being at the time felt.
- The grief-muscles are not very frequently brought into play; and as the
- action is often momentary, it easily escapes observation. Although the
- expression, when observed, is universally and instantly recognized as
- that of grief or anxiety, yet not one person out of a thousand who has
- never studied the subject, is able to say precisely what change passes
- over the sufferer’s face. Hence probably it is that this expression is
- not even alluded to, as far as I have noticed, in any work of fiction,
- with the exception of ‘Red Gauntlet’ and of one other novel; and the
- authoress of the latter, as I am informed, belongs to the famous family
- of actors just alluded to; so that her attention may have been
- specially called to the subject.
- The ancient Greek sculptors were familiar with the expression, as shown
- in the statues of the Laocoon and Arretino; but, as Duchenne remarks,
- they carried the transverse furrows across the whole breadth of the
- forehead, and thus committed a great anatomical mistake: this is
- likewise the case in some modern statues. It is, however, more probable
- that these wonderfully accurate observers intentionally sacrificed
- truth for the sake of beauty, than that they made a mistake; for
- rectangular furrows on the forehead would not have had a grand
- appearance on the marble. The expression, in its fully developed
- condition, is, as far as I can discover, not often represented in
- pictures by the old masters, no doubt owing to the same cause; but a
- lady who is perfectly familiar with this expression, informs me that in
- Fra Angelico’s ‘Descent from the Cross’ in Florence, it is clearly
- exhibited in one of the figures on the right-hand; and I could add a
- few other instances.
- Dr. Crichton Browne, at my request, closely attended to this expression
- in the numerous insane patients under his care in the West Riding
- Asylum; and he is familiar with Duchenne’s photographs of the action of
- the grief-muscles. He informs me that they may constantly be seen in
- energetic action in cases of melancholia, and especially of
- hypochondria; and that the persistent lines or furrows, due to their
- habitual contraction, are characteristic of the physiognomy of the
- insane belonging to these two classes. Dr. Browne carefully observed
- for me during a considerable period three cases of hypochondria, in
- which the grief-muscles were persistently contracted. In one of these,
- a widow, aged 51, fancied that she had lost all her viscera, and that
- her whole body was empty. She wore an expression of great distress, and
- beat her semi-closed hands rhythmically together for hours. The
- grief-muscles were permanently contracted, and the upper eyelids
- arched. This condition lasted for months; she then recovered, and her
- countenance resumed its natural expression. A second case presented
- nearly the same peculiarities, with the addition that the comers of the
- mouth were depressed.
- Mr. Patrick Nicol has also kindly observed for me several cases in the
- Sussex Lunatic Asylum, and has communicated to me full details with
- respect to three of them; but they need not here be given. From his
- observations on melancholic patients, Mr. Nicol concludes that the
- inner ends of the eyebrows are almost always more or less raised, with
- the wrinkles on the forehead more or less plainly marked. In the case
- of one young woman, these wrinkles were observed to be in constant
- slight play or movement. In some cases the comers of the mouth are
- depressed, but often only in a slight degree. Some amount of difference
- in the expression of the several melancholic patients could almost
- always be observed. The eyelids generally droop; and the skin near
- their outer comers and beneath them is wrinkled. The naso-labial fold,
- which runs from the wings of the nostrils to the comers of the mouth,
- and which is so conspicuous in blubbering children, is often plainly
- marked in these patients.
- Although with the insane the grief-muscles often act persistently; yet
- in ordinary cases they are sometimes brought unconsciously into
- momentary action by ludicrously slight causes. A gentleman rewarded a
- young lady by an absurdly small present; she pretended to be offended,
- and as she upbraided him, her eyebrows became extremely oblique, with
- the forehead properly wrinkled. Another young lady and a youth, both in
- the highest spirits, were eagerly talking together with extraordinary
- rapidity; and I noticed that, as often as the young lady was beaten,
- and could not get out her words fast enough, her eyebrows went
- obliquely upwards, and rectangular furrows were formed on her forehead.
- She thus each time hoisted a flag of distress; and this she did
- half-a-dozen times in the course of a few minutes. I made no remark on
- the subject, but on a subsequent occasion I asked her to act on her
- grief-muscles; another girl who was present, and who could do so
- voluntarily, showing her what was intended. She tried repeatedly, but
- utterly failed; yet so slight a cause of distress as not being able to
- talk quickly enough, sufficed to bring these muscles over and over
- again into energetic action.
- The expression of grief, due to the contraction of the grief-muscles,
- is by no means confined to Europeans, but appears to be common to all
- the races of mankind. I have, at least, received trustworthy accounts
- in regard to Hindoos, Dhangars (one of the aboriginal hill-tribes of
- India, and therefore belonging to a quite distinct race from the
- Hindoos), Malays, Negroes and Australians. With respect to the latter,
- two observers answer my query in the affirmative, but enter into no
- details. Mr. Taplin, however, appends to my descriptive remarks the
- words “this is exact.” With respect to negroes, the lady who told me of
- Fra Angelico’s picture, saw a negro towing a boat on the Nile, and as
- he encountered an obstruction, she observed his grief-muscles in strong
- action, with the middle of the forehead well wrinkled. Mr. Geach
- watched a Malay man in Malacca, with the comers of his mouth much
- depressed, the eyebrows oblique, with deep short grooves on the
- forehead. This expression lasted for a very short time; and Mr. Geach
- remarks it “was a strange one, very much like a person about to cry at
- some great loss.”
- In India Mr. H. Erskine found that the natives were familiar with this
- expression; and Mr. J. Scott, of the Botanic Gardens, Calcutta, has
- obligingly sent me a full description of two cases. He observed during
- some time, himself unseen, a very young Dhangar woman from Nagpore, the
- wife of one of the gardeners, nursing her baby who was at the point of
- death; and he distinctly saw the eyebrows raised at the inner comers,
- the eyelids drooping, the forehead wrinkled in the middle, the mouth
- slightly open, with the comers much depressed. He then came from behind
- a screen of plants and spoke to the poor woman, who started, burst into
- a bitter flood of tears, and besought him to cure her baby. The second
- case was that of a Hindustani man, who from illness and poverty was
- compelled to sell his favourite goat. After receiving the money, he
- repeatedly looked at the money in his hand and then at the goat, as if
- doubting whether he would not return it. He went to the goat, which was
- tied up ready to be led away, and the animal reared up and licked his
- hands. His eyes then wavered from side to side; his “mouth was
- partially closed, with the corners very decidedly depressed.” At last
- the poor man seemed to make up his mind that he must part with his
- goat, and then, as Mr. Scott saw, the eyebrows became slightly oblique,
- with the characteristic puckering or swelling at the inner ends, but
- the wrinkles on the forehead were not present. The man stood thus for a
- minute, then heaving a deep sigh, burst into tears, raised up his two
- hands, blessed the goat, turned round, and without looking again, went
- away.
- _On the cause of the obliquity of the eyebrows under suffering_.—During
- several years no expression seemed to me so utterly perplexing as this
- which we are here considering. Why should grief or anxiety cause the
- central fasciae alone of the frontal muscle together with those round
- the eyes, to contract? Here we seem to have a complex movement for the
- sole purpose of expressing grief; and yet it is a comparatively rare
- expression, and often overlooked. I believe the explanation is not so
- difficult as it at first appears. Dr. Duchenne gives a photograph of
- the young man before referred to, who, when looking upwards at a
- strongly illuminated surface, involuntarily contracted his
- grief-muscles in an exaggerated manner. I had entirely forgotten this
- photograph, when on a very bright day with the sun behind me, I met,
- whilst on horseback, a girl whose eyebrows, as she looked up at me,
- became extremely oblique, with the proper furrows on her forehead. I
- have observed the same movement under similar circumstances on several
- subsequent occasions. On my return home I made three of my children,
- without giving them any clue to my object, look as long and as
- attentively as they could, at the summit of a tall tree standing
- against an extremely bright sky. With all three, the orbicular,
- corrugator, and pyramidal muscles were energetically contracted,
- through reflex action, from the excitement of the retina, so that their
- eyes might be protected from the bright light. But they tried their
- utmost to look upwards; and now a curious struggle, with spasmodic
- twitchings, could be observed between the whole or only the central
- portion of the frontal muscle, and the several muscles which serve to
- lower the eyebrows and close the eyelids. The involuntary contraction
- of the pyramidal caused the basal part of their noses to be
- transversely and deeply wrinkled. In one of the three children, the
- whole eyebrows were momentarily raised and lowered by the alternate
- contraction of the whole frontal muscle and of the muscles surrounding
- the eyes, so that the whole breadth of the forehead was alternately
- wrinkled and smoothed. In the other two children the forehead became
- wrinkled in the middle part alone, rectangular furrows being thus
- produced; and the eyebrows were rendered oblique, with their inner
- extremities puckered and swollen,—in the one child in a slight degree,
- in the other in a strongly marked manner. This difference in the
- obliquity of the eyebrows apparently depended on a difference in their
- general mobility, and in the strength of the pyramidal muscles. In both
- these cases the eyebrows and forehead were acted on under the influence
- of a strong light, in precisely the same manner, in every
- characteristic detail, as under the influence of grief or anxiety.
- Duchenne states that the pyramidal muscle of the nose is less under the
- control of the will than are the other muscles round the eyes. He
- remarks that the young man who could so well act on his grief-muscles,
- as well as on most of his other facial muscles, could not contract the
- pyramidals.[705] This power, however, no doubt differs in different
- persons. The pyramidal muscle serves to draw down the skin of the
- forehead between the eyebrows, together with their inner extremities.
- The central fasciae of the frontal are the antagonists of the
- pyramidal; and if the action of the latter is to be specially checked,
- these central fasciae must be contracted. So that with persons having
- powerful pyramidal muscles, if there is under the influence of a bright
- light an unconscious desire to prevent the lowering of the eyebrows,
- the central fasciae of the frontal muscle must be brought into play;
- and their contraction, if sufficiently strong to overmaster the
- pyramidals, together with the contraction of the corrugator and
- orbicular muscles, will act in the manner just described on the
- eyebrows and forehead.
- When children scream or cry out, they contract, as we know, the
- orbicular, corrugator, and pyramidal muscles, primarily for the sake of
- compressing their eyes, and thus protecting them from being gorged with
- blood, and secondarily through habit. I therefore expected to find with
- children, that when they endeavoured either to prevent a crying-fit
- from coming on, or to stop crying, they would cheek the contraction of
- the above-named muscles, in the same manner as when looking upwards at
- a bright light; and consequently that the central fasciae of the
- frontal muscle would often be brought into play. Accordingly, I began
- myself to observe children at such times, and asked others, including
- some medical men, to do the same. It is necessary to observe carefully,
- as the peculiar opposed action of these muscles is not nearly so plain
- in children, owing to their foreheads not easily wrinkling, as in
- adults. But I soon found that the grief-muscles were very frequently
- brought into distinct action on these occasions. It would be
- superfluous to give all the cases which have been observed; and I will
- specify only a few. A little girl, a year and a half old, was teased by
- some other children, and before bursting into tears her eyebrows became
- decidedly oblique. With an older girl the same obliquity was observed,
- with the inner ends of the eyebrows plainly puckered; and at the same
- time the corners of the mouth were drawn downwards. As soon as she
- burst into tears, the features all changed and this peculiar expression
- vanished. Again, after a little boy had been vaccinated, which made him
- scream and cry violently, the surgeon gave him an orange brought for
- the purpose, and this pleased the child much; as he stopped crying all
- the characteristic movements were observed, including the formation of
- rectangular wrinkles in the middle of the forehead. Lastly, I met on
- the road a little girl three or four years old, who had been frightened
- by a dog, and when I asked her what was the matter, she stopped
- whimpering, and her eyebrows instantly became oblique to an
- extraordinary degree.
- Here then, as I cannot doubt, we have the key to the problem why the
- central fasciae of the frontal muscle and the muscles round the eyes
- contract in opposition to each other under the influence of
- grief;—whether their contraction be prolonged, as with the melancholic
- insane, or momentary, from some trifling cause of distress. We have all
- of us, as infants, repeatedly contracted our orbicular, corrugator, and
- pyramidal muscles, in order to protect our eyes whilst screaming; our
- progenitors before us have done the same during many generations; and
- though with advancing years we easily prevent, when feeling distressed,
- the utterance of screams, we cannot from long habit always prevent a
- slight contraction of the above-named muscles; nor indeed do we observe
- their contraction in ourselves, or attempt to stop it, if slight. But
- the pyramidal muscles seem to be less under the command of the will
- than the other related muscles; and if they be well developed, their
- contraction can be checked only by the antagonistic contraction of the
- central fasciae of the frontal muscle. The result which necessarily
- follows, if these fasciae contract energetically, is the oblique
- drawing up of the eyebrows, the puckering of their inner ends, and the
- formation of rectangular furrows on the middle of the forehead. As
- children and women cry much more freely than men, and as grown-up
- persons of both sexes rarely weep except from mental distress, we can
- understand why the grief-muscles are more frequently seen in action, as
- I believe to be the case, with children and women than with men; and
- with adults of both sexes from mental distress alone. In some of the
- cases before recorded, as in that of the poor Dhangar woman and of the
- Hindustani man, the action of the grief-muscles was quickly followed by
- bitter weeping. In all cases of distress, whether great or small, our
- brains tend through long habit to send an order to certain muscles to
- contract, as if we were still infants on the point of screaming out;
- but this order we, by the wondrous power of the will, and through
- habit, are able partially to counteract; although this is effected
- unconsciously, as far as the means of counteraction are concerned.
- _On the depression of the corners of the mouth_.—This action is
- effected by the _depressores anguili oris_ (see letter K in figs. 1 and
- 2). The fibres of this muscle diverge downwards, with the upper
- convergent ends attached round the angles of the mouth, and to the
- lower lip a little way within the angles.[706] Some of the fibres
- appear to be antagonistic to the great zygomatic muscle, and others to
- the several muscles running to the outer part of the upper lip. The
- contraction of this muscle draws downwards and outwards the corners of
- the mouth, including the outer part of the upper lip, and even in a
- slight degree the wings of the nostrils. When the mouth is closed and
- this muscle acts, the commissure or line of junction of the two lips
- forms a curved line with the concavity downwards,[707] and the lips
- themselves are generally somewhat protruded, especially the lower one.
- The mouth in this state is well represented in the two photographs
- (Plate II., figs. 6 and 7) by Mr. Rejlander. The upper boy (fig. 6) had
- just stopped crying, after receiving a slap on the face from another
- boy; and the right moment was seized for photographing him.
- The expression of low spirits, grief or dejection, due to the
- contraction of this muscle has been noticed by every one who has
- written on the subject. To say that a person “is down in the mouth,” is
- synonymous with saying that he is out of spirits. The depression of the
- corners may often be seen, as already stated on the authority of Dr.
- Crichton Browne and Mr. Nicol, with the melancholic insane, and was
- well exhibited in some photographs sent to me by the former gentleman,
- of patients with a strong tendency to suicide. It has been observed
- with men belonging to various races, namely with Hindoos, the dark
- hill-tribes of India, Malays, and, as the Rev. Mr. Hagenauer informs
- me, with the aborigines of Australia.
- When infants scream they firmly contract the muscles round their eyes,
- and this draws up the upper lip; and as they have to keep their mouths
- widely open, the depressor muscles running to the corners are likewise
- brought into strong action. This generally, but not invariably, causes
- a slight angular bend in the lower lip on both sides, near the corners
- of the mouth. The result of the upper and lower lip being thus acted on
- is that the mouth assumes a squarish outline. The contraction of the
- depressor muscle is best seen in infants when not screaming violently,
- and especially just before they begin, or when they cease to scream.
- Their little faces then acquire an extremely piteous expression, as I
- continually observed with my own infants between the ages of about six
- weeks and two or three months. Sometimes, when they are struggling
- against a crying-fit, the outline of the mouth is curved in so
- exaggerated a manner as to be like a horseshoe; and the expression of
- misery then becomes a ludicrous caricature.
- The explanation of the contraction of this muscle, under the influence
- of low spirits or dejection, apparently follows from the same general
- principles as in the case of the obliquity of the eyebrows. Dr.
- Duchenne informs me that he concludes from his observations, now
- prolonged during many years, that this is one of the facial muscles
- which is least under the control of the will. This fact may indeed be
- inferred from what has just been stated with respect to infants when
- doubtfully beginning to cry, or endeavouring to stop crying; for they
- then generally command all the other facial muscles more effectually
- than they do the depressors of the corners of the mouth. Two excellent
- observers who had no theory on the subject, one of them a surgeon,
- carefully watched for me some older children and women as with some
- opposed struggling they very gradually approached the point of bursting
- out into tears; and both observers felt sure that the depressors began
- to act before any of the other muscles. Now as the depressors have been
- repeatedly brought into strong action during infancy in many
- generations, nerve-force will tend to flow, on the principle of long
- associated habit, to these muscles as well as to various other facial
- muscles, whenever in after life even a slight feeling of distress is
- experienced. But as the depressors are somewhat less under the control
- of the will than most of the other muscles, we might expect that they
- would often slightly contract, whilst the others remained passive. It
- is remarkable how small a depression of the corners of the mouth gives
- to the countenance an expression of low spirits or dejection, so that
- an extremely slight contraction of these muscles would be sufficient to
- betray this state of mind.
- I may here mention a trifling observation, as it will serve to sum up
- our present subject. An old lady with a comfortable but absorbed
- expression sat nearly opposite to me in a railway carriage. Whilst I
- was looking at her, I saw that her _depressores anguli oris_ became
- very slightly, yet decidedly, contracted; but as her countenance
- remained as placid as ever, I reflected how meaningless was this
- contraction, and how easily one might be deceived. The thought had
- hardly occurred to me when I saw that her eyes suddenly became suffused
- with tears almost to overflowing, and her whole countenance fell. There
- could now be no doubt that some painful recollection, perhaps that of a
- long-lost child, was passing through her mind. As soon as her sensorium
- was thus affected, certain nerve-cells from long habit instantly
- transmitted an order to all the respiratory muscles, and to those round
- the mouth, to prepare for a fit of crying. But the order was
- countermanded by the will, or rather by a later acquired habit, and all
- the muscles were obedient, excepting in a slight degree the
- _depressores anguli oris_. The mouth was not even opened; the
- respiration was not hurried; and no muscle was affected except those
- which draw down the corners of the mouth.
- As soon as the mouth of this lady began, involuntarily and
- unconsciously on her part, to assume the proper form for a crying-fit,
- we may feel almost sure that some nerve-influence would have been
- transmitted through the long accustomed channels to the various
- respiratory muscles, as well as to those round the eyes, and to the
- vaso-motor centre which governs the supply of blood sent to the
- lacrymal glands. Of this latter fact we have indeed clear evidence in
- her eyes becoming slightly suffused with tears; and we can understand
- this, as the lacrymal glands are less under the control of the will
- than the facial muscles. No doubt there existed at the same time some
- tendency in the muscles round the eyes at contract, as if for the sake
- of protecting them from being gorged with blood, but this contraction
- was completely overmastered, and her brow remained unruffled. Had the
- pyramidal, corrugator, and orbicular muscles been as little obedient to
- the will, as they are in many persons, they would have been slightly
- acted on; and then the central fasciae of the frontal muscle would have
- contracted in antagonism, and her eyebrows would have become oblique,
- with rectangular furrows on her forehead. Her countenance would then
- have expressed still more plainly than it did a state of dejection, or
- rather one of grief.
- Through steps such as these we can understand how it is, that as soon
- as some melancholy thought passes through the brain, there occurs a
- just perceptible drawing down of the corners of the mouth, or a slight
- raising up of the inner ends of the eyebrows, or both movements
- combined, and immediately afterwards a slight suffusion of tears. A
- thrill of nerve-force is transmitted along several habitual channels,
- and produces an effect on any point where the will has not acquired
- through long habit much power of interference. The above actions may be
- considered as rudimental vestiges of the screaming-fits, which are so
- frequent and prolonged during infancy. In this case, as well as in many
- others, the links are indeed wonderful which connect cause and effect
- in giving rise to various expressions on the human countenance; and
- they explain to us the meaning of certain movements, which we
- involuntarily and unconsciously perform, whenever certain transitory
- emotions pass through our minds.
- CHAPTER VIII. JOY, HIGH SPIRITS, LOVE, TENDER FEELINGS, DEVOTION.
- Laughter primarily the expression of joy—Ludicrous ideas—Movements of
- the features during laughter—Nature of the sound produced—The secretion
- of tears during loud laughter—Gradation from loud laughter to gentle
- smiling—High spirits—The expression of love—Tender feelings—Devotion.
- Joy, when intense, leads to various purposeless movements—to dancing
- about, clapping the hands, stamping, &c., and to loud laughter.
- Laughter seems primarily to be the expression of mere joy or happiness.
- We clearly see this in children at play, who are almost incessantly
- laughing. With young persons past childhood, when they are in high
- spirits, there is always much meaningless laughter. The laughter of the
- gods is described by Homer as “the exuberance of their celestial joy
- after their daily banquet.” A man smiles—and smiling, as we shall see,
- graduates into laughter—at meeting an old friend in the street, as he
- does at any trifling pleasure, such as smelling a sweet perfume.[801]
- Laura Bridgman, from her blindness and deafness, could not have
- acquired any expression through imitation, yet when a letter from a
- beloved friend was communicated to her by gesture-language, she
- “laughed and clapped her hands, and the colour mounted to her cheeks.”
- On other occasions she has been seen to stamp for joy.[802]
- Idiots and imbecile persons likewise afford good evidence that laughter
- or smiling primarily expresses mere happiness or joy. Dr. Crichton
- Browne, to whom, as on so many other occasions, I am indebted for the
- results of his wide experience, informs me that with idiots laughter is
- the most prevalent and frequent of all the emotional expressions. Many
- idiots are morose, passionate, restless, in a painful state of mind, or
- utterly stolid, and these never laugh. Others frequently laugh in a
- quite senseless manner. Thus an idiot boy, incapable of speech,
- complained to Dr. Browne, by the aid of signs, that another boy in the
- asylum had given him a black eye; and this was accompanied by
- “explosions of laughter and with his face covered with the broadest
- smiles.” There is another large class of idiots who are persistently
- joyous and benign, and who are constantly laughing or smiling.[803]
- Their countenances often exhibit a stereotyped smile; their joyousness
- is increased, and they grin, chuckle, or giggle, whenever food is
- placed before them, or when they are caressed, are shown bright
- colours, or hear music. Some of them laugh more than usual when they
- walk about, or attempt any muscular exertion. The joyousness of most of
- these idiots cannot possibly be associated, as Dr. Browne remarks, with
- any distinct ideas: they simply feel pleasure, and express it by
- laughter or smiles. With imbeciles rather higher in the scale, personal
- vanity seems to be the commonest cause of laughter, and next to this,
- pleasure arising from the approbation of their conduct.
- With grown-up persons laughter is excited by causes considerably
- different from those which suffice during childhood; but this remark
- hardly applies to smiling. Laughter in this respect is analogous with
- weeping, which with adults is almost confined to mental distress,
- whilst with children it is excited by bodily pain or any suffering, as
- well as by fear or rage. Many curious discussions have been written on
- the causes of laughter with grown-up persons. The subject is extremely
- complex. Something incongruous or unaccountable, exciting surprise and
- some sense of superiority in the laugher, who must be in a happy frame
- of mind, seems to be the commonest cause.[804] The circumstances must
- not be of a momentous nature: no poor man would laugh or smile on
- suddenly hearing that a large fortune had been bequeathed to him. If
- the mind is strongly excited by pleasurable feelings, and any little
- unexpected event or thought occurs, then, as Mr. Herbert Spencer
- remarks,[805] “a large amount of nervous energy, instead of being
- allowed to expend itself in producing an equivalent amount of the new
- thoughts and emotion which were nascent, is suddenly checked in its
- flow.”... “The excess must discharge itself in some other direction,
- and there results an efflux through the motor nerves to various classes
- of the muscles, producing the half-convulsive actions we term
- laughter.” An observation, bearing on this point, was made by a
- correspondent during the recent siege of Paris, namely, that the German
- soldiers, after strong excitement from exposure to extreme danger, were
- particularly apt to burst out into loud laughter at the smallest joke.
- So again when young children are just beginning to cry, an unexpected
- event will sometimes suddenly turn their crying into laughter, which
- apparently serves equally well to expend their superfluous nervous
- energy.
- The imagination is sometimes said to be tickled by a ludicrous idea;
- and this so-called tickling of the mind is curiously analogous with
- that of the body. Every one knows how immoderately children laugh, and
- how their whole bodies are convulsed when they are tickled. The
- anthropoid apes, as we have seen, likewise utter a reiterated sound,
- corresponding with our laughter, when they are tickled, especially
- under the armpits. I touched with a bit of paper the sole of the foot
- of one of my infants, when only seven days old, and it was suddenly
- jerked away and the toes curled about, as in an older child. Such
- movements, as well as laughter from being tickled, are manifestly
- reflex actions; and this is likewise shown by the minute unstriped
- muscles, which serve to erect the separate hairs on the body,
- contracting near a tickled surface.[806] Yet laughter from a ludicrous
- idea, though involuntary, cannot be called a strictly reflex action. In
- this case, and in that of laughter from being tickled, the mind must be
- in a pleasurable condition; a young child, if tickled by a strange man,
- would scream from fear. The touch must be light, and an idea or event,
- to be ludicrous, must not be of grave import. The parts of the body
- which are most easily tickled are those which are not commonly touched,
- such as the armpits or between the toes, or parts such as the soles of
- the feet, which are habitually touched by a broad surface; but the
- surface on which we sit offers a marked exception to this rule.
- According to Gratiolet,[807] certain nerves are much more sensitive to
- tickling than others. From the fact that a child can hardly tickle
- itself, or in a much less degree than when tickled by another person,
- it seems that the precise point to be touched must not be known; so
- with the mind, something unexpected—a novel or incongruous idea which
- breaks through an habitual train of thought—appears to be a strong
- element in the ludicrous.
- The sound of laughter is produced by a deep inspiration followed by
- short, interrupted, spasmodic contractions of the chest, and especially
- of the diaphragm.[808] Hence we hear of “laughter holding both his
- sides.” From the shaking of the body, the head nods to and fro. The
- lower jaw often quivers up and down, as is likewise the case with some
- species of baboons, when they are much pleased.
- Moderate Laughter and Smiling. Plate III
- During laughter the mouth is opened more or less widely, with the
- corners drawn much backwards, as well as a little upwards; and the
- upper lip is somewhat raised. The drawing back of the corners is best
- seen in moderate laughter, and especially in a broad smile—the latter
- epithet showing how the mouth is widened. In the accompanying figs.
- 1-3, Plate III., different degrees of moderate laughter and smiling
- have been photographed. The figure of the little girl, with the hat is
- by Dr. Wallich, and the expression was a genuine one; the other two are
- by Mr. Rejlander. Dr. Duchenne repeatedly insists[809] that, under the
- emotion of joy, the mouth is acted on exclusively by the great
- zygomatic muscles, which serve to draw the corners backwards and
- upwards; but judging from the manner in which the upper teeth are
- always exposed during laughter and broad smiling, as well as from my
- own sensations, I cannot doubt that some of the muscles running to the
- upper lip are likewise brought into moderate action. The upper and
- lower orbicular muscles of the eyes are at the same time more or less
- contracted; and there is an intimate connection, as explained in the
- chapter on weeping, between the orbiculars, especially the lower ones
- and some of the muscles running to the upper lip. Henle remarks[810] on
- this head, that when a man closely shuts one eye he cannot avoid
- retracting the upper lip on the same side; conversely, if any one will
- place his finger on his lower eyelid, and then uncover his upper
- incisors as much as possible, he will feel, as his upper lip is drawn
- strongly upwards, that the muscles of the lower eyelid contract. In
- Henle’s drawing, given in woodcut, fig. 2, the _musculus malaris_ (H)
- which runs to the upper lip may be seen to form an almost integral part
- of the lower orbicular muscle.
- Dr. Duchenne has given a large photograph of an old man (reduced on
- Plate III. fig 4), in his usual passive condition, and another of the
- same man (fig. 5), naturally smiling. The latter was instantly
- recognized by every one to whom it was shown as true to nature. He has
- also given, as an example of an unnatural or false smile, another
- photograph (fig. 6) of the same old man, with the corners of his mouth
- strongly retracted by the galvanization of the great zygomatic muscles.
- That the expression is not natural is clear, for I showed this
- photograph to twenty-four persons, of whom three could not in the least
- tell what was meant, whilst the others, though they perceived that the
- expression was of the nature of a smile, answered in such words as “a
- wicked joke,” “trying to laugh,” “grinning laughter.... half-amazed
- laughter,” &c. Dr. Duchenne attributes the falseness of the expression
- altogether to the orbicular muscles of the lower eyelids not being
- sufficiently contracted; for he justly lays great stress on their
- contraction in the expression of joy. No doubt there is much truth in
- this view, but not, as it appears to me, the whole truth. The
- contraction of the lower orbiculars is always accompanied, as we have
- seen, by the drawing up of the upper lip. Had the upper lip, in fig. 6,
- been thus acted on to a slight extent, its curvature would have been
- less rigid, the naso-labial farrow would have been slightly different,
- and the whole expression would, as I believe, have been more natural,
- independently of the more conspicuous effect from the stronger
- contraction of the lower eyelids. The corrugator muscle, moreover, in
- fig. 6, is too much contracted, causing a frown; and this muscle never
- acts under the influence of joy except during strongly pronounced or
- violent laughter.
- By the drawing backwards and upwards of the corners of the mouth,
- through the contraction of the great zygomatic muscles, and by the
- raising of the upper lip, the cheeks are drawn upwards. Wrinkles are
- thus formed under the eyes, and, with old people, at their outer ends;
- and these are highly characteristic of laughter or smiling. As a gentle
- smile increases into a strong one, or into a laugh, every one may feel
- and see, if he will attend to his own sensations and look at himself in
- a mirror, that as the upper lip is drawn up and the lower orbiculars
- contract, the wrinkles in the lower eyelids and those beneath the eyes
- are much strengthened or increased. At the same time, as I have
- repeatedly observed, the eyebrows are slightly lowered, which shows
- that the upper as well as the lower orbiculars contract at least to
- some degree, though this passes unperecived, as far as our sensations
- are concerned. If the original photograph of the old man, with his
- countenance in its usual placid state (fig. 4), be compared with that
- (fig. 5) in which he is naturally smiling, it may be seen that the
- eyebrows in the latter are a little lowered. I presume that this is
- owing to the upper orbiculars being impelled, through the force of
- long-associated habit, to act to a certain extent in concert with the
- lower orbiculars, which themselves contract in connection with the
- drawing up of the upper lip.
- The tendency in the zygomatic muscles to contract under pleasurable
- emotions is shown by a curious fact, communicated to me by Dr. Browne,
- with respect to patients suffering from GENERAL PARALYSIS OF THE
- INSANE.[811] “In this malady there is almost invariably
- optimism—delusions as to wealth, rank, grandeur—insane joyousness,
- benevolence, and profusion, while its very earliest physical symptom is
- trembling at the corners of the mouth and at the outer corners of the
- eyes. This is a well-recognized fact. Constant tremulous agitation of
- the inferior palpebral and great zygomatic muscles is pathognomic of
- the earlier stages of general paralysis. The countenance has a pleased
- and benevolent expression. As the disease advances other muscles become
- involved, but until complete fatuity is reached, the prevailing
- expression is that of feeble benevolence.”
- As in laughing and broadly smiling the cheeks and upper lip are much
- raised, the nose appears to be shortened, and the skin on the bridge
- becomes finely wrinkled in transverse lines, with other oblique
- longitudinal lines on the sides. The upper front teeth are commonly
- exposed. A well-marked naso-labial fold is formed, which runs from the
- wing of each nostril to the corner of the mouth; and this fold is often
- double in old persons.
- A bright and sparkling eye is as characteristic of a pleased or amused
- state of mind, as is the retraction of the corners of the mouth and
- upper lip with the wrinkles thus produced. Even the eyes of
- microcephalous idiots, who are so degraded that they never learn to
- speak, brighten slightly when they are pleased.[812] Under extreme
- laughter the eyes are too much suffused with tears to sparkle; but the
- moisture squeezed out of the glands during moderate laughter or smiling
- may aid in giving them lustre; though this must be of altogether
- subordinate importance, as they become dull from grief, though they are
- then often moist. Their brightness seems to be chiefly due to their
- tenseness,[813] owing to the contraction of the orbicular muscles and
- to the pressure of the raised cheeks. But, according to Dr. Piderit,
- who has discussed this point more fully than any other writer,[814] the
- tenseness may be largely attributed to the eyeballs becoming filled
- with blood and other fluids, from the acceleration of the circulation,
- consequent on the excitement of pleasure. He remarks on the contrast in
- the appearance of the eyes of a hectic patient with a rapid
- circulation, and of a man suffering from cholera with almost all the
- fluids of his body drained from him. Any cause which lowers the
- circulation deadens the eye. I remember seeing a man utterly prostrated
- by prolonged and severe exertion during a very hot day, and a bystander
- compared his eyes to those of a boiled codfish.
- To return to the sounds produced during laughter. We can see in a vague
- manner how the utterance of sounds of some kind would naturally become
- associated with a pleasurable state of mind; for throughout a large
- part of the animal kingdom vocal or instrumental sounds are employed
- either as a call or as a charm by one sex for the other. They are also
- employed as the means for a joyful meeting between the parents and
- their offspring, and between the attached members of the same social
- community. But why the sounds which man utters when he is pleased have
- the peculiar reiterated character of laughter we do not know.
- Nevertheless we can see that they would naturally be as different as
- possible from the screams or cries of distress; and as in the
- production of the latter, the expirations are prolonged and continuous,
- with the inspirations short and interrupted, so it might perhaps have
- been expected with the sounds uttered from joy, that the expirations
- would have been short and broken with the inspirations prolonged; and
- this is the case.
- It is an equally obscure point why the corners of the mouth are
- retracted and the upper lip raised during ordinary laughter. The mouth
- must not be opened to its utmost extent, for when this occurs during a
- paroxysm of excessive laughter hardly any sound is emitted; or it
- changes its tone and seems to come from deep down in the throat. The
- respiratory muscles, and even those of the limbs, are at the same time
- thrown into rapid vibratory movements. The lower jaw often partakes of
- this movement, and this would tend to prevent the mouth from being
- widely opened. But as a full volume of sound has to be poured forth,
- the orifice of the mouth must be large; and it is perhaps to gain this
- end that the corners are retracted and the upper lip raised. Although
- we can hardly account for the shape of the mouth during laughter, which
- leads to wrinkles being formed beneath the eyes, nor for the peculiar
- reiterated sound of laughter, nor for the quivering of the jaws,
- nevertheless we may infer that all these effects are due to some common
- cause. For they are all characteristic and expressive of a pleased
- state of mind in various kinds of monkeys.
- A graduated series can be followed from violent to moderate laughter,
- to a broad smile, to a gentle smile, and to the expression of mere
- cheerfulness. During excessive laughter the whole body is often thrown
- backward and shakes, or is almost convulsed; the respiration is much
- disturbed; the head and face become gorged with blood, with the veins
- distended; and the orbicular muscles are spasmodically contracted in
- order to protect the eyes. Tears are freely shed. Hence, as formerly
- remarked, it is scarcely possible to point out any difference between
- the tear-stained face of a person after a paroxysm of excessive
- laughter and after a bitter crying-fit.[815] It is probably due to the
- close similarity of the spasmodic movements caused by these widely
- different emotions that hysteric patients alternately cry and laugh
- with violence, and that young children sometimes pass suddenly from the
- one to the other state. Mr. Swinhoe informs me that he has often seen
- the Chinese, when suffering from deep grief, burst out into hysterical
- fits of laughter.
- I was anxious to know whether tears are freely shed during excessive
- laughter by most of the races of men, and I hear from my correspondents
- that this is the case. One instance was observed with the Hindoos, and
- they themselves said that it often occurred. So it is with the Chinese.
- The women of a wild tribe of Malays in the Malacca peninsula, sometimes
- shed tears when they laugh heartily, though this seldom occurs. With
- the Dyaks of Borneo it must frequently be the case, at least with the
- women, for I hear from the Rajah C. Brooke that it is a common
- expression with them to say “we nearly made tears from laughter.” The
- aborigines of Australia express their emotions freely, and they are
- described by my correspondents as jumping about and clapping their
- hands for joy, and as often roaring with laughter. No less than four
- observers have seen their eyes freely watering on such occasions; and
- in one instance the tears rolled down their cheeks. Mr. Bulmer, a
- missionary in a remote part of Victoria, remarks, “that they have a
- keen sense of the ridiculous; they are excellent mimics, and when one
- of them is able to imitate the peculiarities of some absent member of
- the tribe, it is very common to hear all in the camp convulsed with
- laughter.” With Europeans hardly anything excites laughter so easily as
- mimicry; and it is rather curious to find the same fact with the
- savages of Australia, who constitute one of the most distinct races in
- the world.
- In Southern Africa with two tribes of Kafirs, especially with the
- women, their eyes often fill with tears during laughter. Gaika, the
- brother of the chief Sandilli, answers my query on this head, with the
- words, “Yes, that is their common practice.” Sir Andrew Smith has seen
- the painted face of a Hottentot woman all furrowed with tears after a
- fit of laughter. In Northern Africa, with the Abyssinians, tears are
- secreted under the same circumstances. Lastly, in North America, the
- same fact has been observed in a remarkably savage and isolated tribe,
- but chiefly with the women; in another tribe it was observed only on a
- single occasion.
- Excessive laughter, as before remarked, graduates into moderate
- laughter. In this latter case the muscles round the eyes are much less
- contracted, and there is little or no frowning. Between a gentle laugh
- and a broad smile there is hardly any difference, excepting that in
- smiling no reiterated sound is uttered, though a single rather strong
- expiration, or slight noise—a rudiment of a laugh—may often be heard at
- the commencement of a smile. On a moderately smiling countenance the
- contraction of the upper orbicular muscles can still just be traced by
- a slight lowering of the eyebrows. The contraction of the lower
- orbicular and palpebral muscles is much plainer, and is shown by the
- wrinkling of the lower eyelids and of the skin beneath them, together
- with a slight drawing up of the upper lip. From the broadest smile we
- pass by the finest steps into the gentlest one. In this latter case the
- features are moved in a much less degree, and much more slowly, and the
- mouth is kept closed. The curvature of the naso-labial furrow is also
- slightly different in the two cases. We thus see that no abrupt line of
- demarcation can be drawn between the movement of the features during
- the most violent laughter and a very faint smile.[816]
- A smile, therefore, may be said to be the first stage in the
- development of a laugh. But a different and more probable view may be
- suggested; namely, that the habit of uttering load reiterated sounds
- from a sense of pleasure, first led to the retraction of the corners of
- the mouth and of the upper lip, and to the contraction of the orbicular
- muscles; and that now, through association and long-continued habit,
- the same muscles are brought into slight play whenever any cause
- excites in us a feeling which, if stronger, would have led to laughter;
- and the result is a smile.
- Whether we look at laughter as the full development of a smile, or, as
- is more probable, at a gentle smile as the last trace of a habit,
- firmly fixed during many generations, of laughing whenever we are
- joyful, we can follow in our infants the gradual passage of the one
- into the other. It is well known to those who have the charge of young
- infants, that it is difficult to feel sure when certain movements about
- their mouths are really expressive; that is, when they really smile.
- Hence I carefully watched my own infants. One of them at the age of
- forty-five days, and being at the time in a happy frame of mind,
- smiled; that is, the corners of the mouth were retracted, and
- simultaneously the eyes became decidedly bright. I observed the same
- thing on the following day; but on the third day the child was not
- quite well and there was no trace of a smile, and this renders it
- probable that the previous smiles were real. Eight days subsequently
- and during the next succeeding week, it was remarkable how his eyes
- brightened whenever he smiled, and his nose became at the same time
- transversely wrinkled. This was now accompanied by a little bleating
- noise, which perhaps represented a laugh. At the age of 113 days these
- little noises, which were always made during expiration, assumed a
- slightly different character, and were more broken or interrupted, as
- in sobbing; and this was certainly incipient laughter. The change in
- tone seemed to me at the time to be connected with the greater lateral
- extension of the mouth as the smiles became broader.
- In a second infant the first real smile was observed at about the same
- age, viz. forty-five days; and in a third, at a somewhat earlier age.
- The second infant, when sixty-five days old, smiled much more broadly
- and plainly than did the one first mentioned at the same age; and even
- at this early age uttered noises very like laughter. In this gradual
- acquirement, by infants, of the habit of laughing, we have a case in
- some degree analogous to that of weeping. As practice is requisite with
- the ordinary movements of the body, such as walking, so it seems to be
- with laughing and weeping. The art of screaming, on the other hand,
- from being of service to infants, has become finely developed from the
- earliest days.
- _High spirits, cheerfulness_.—A man in high spirits, though he may not
- actually smile, commonly exhibits some tendency to the retraction of
- the corners of his mouth. From the excitement of pleasure, the
- circulation becomes more rapid; the eyes are bright, and the colour of
- the face rises. The brain, being stimulated by the increased flow of
- blood, reacts on the mental powers; lively ideas pass still more
- rapidly through the mind, and the affections are warmed. I heard a
- child, a little under four years old, when asked what was meant by
- being in good spirits, answer, “It is laughing, talking, and kissing.”
- It would be difficult to give a truer and more practical definition. A
- man in this state holds his body erect, his head upright, and his eyes
- open. There is no drooping of the features, and no contraction of the
- eyebrows. On the contrary, the frontal muscle, as Moreau observes,[817]
- tends to contract slightly; and this smooths the brow, removes every
- trace of a frown, arches the eyebrows a little, and raises the eyelids.
- Hence the Latin phrase, _exporrigere frontem_—to unwrinkle the
- brow—means, to be cheerful or merry. The whole expression of a man in
- good spirits is exactly the opposite of that of one suffering from
- sorrow. According to Sir C. Bell, “In all the exhilarating emotions the
- eyebrows, eyelids, the nostrils, and the angles of the mouth are
- raised. In the depressing passions it is the reverse.” Under the
- influence of the latter the brow is heavy, the eyelids, cheeks, mouth,
- and whole head droop; the eyes are dull; the countenance pallid, and
- the respiration slow. In joy the face expands, in grief it lengthens.
- Whether the principle of antithesis has here come into play in
- producing these opposite expressions, in aid of the direct causes which
- have been specified and which are sufficiently plain, I will not
- pretend to say.
- With all the races of man the expression of good spirit appears to be
- the same, and is easily recognized. My informants, from various parts
- of the Old and New Worlds, answer in the affirmative to my queries on
- this head, and they give some particulars with respect to Hindoos,
- Malays, and New Zealanders. The brightness of the eyes of the
- Australians has struck four observers, and the same fact has been
- noticed with Hindoos, New Zealanders, and the Dyaks of Borneo.
- Savages sometimes express their satisfaction not only by smiling, but
- by gestures derived from the pleasure of eating. Thus Mr. Wedgwood[818]
- quotes Petherick that the negroes on the Upper Nile began a general
- rubbing of their bellies when he displayed his beads; and Leichhardt
- says that the Australians smacked and clacked their mouths at the sight
- of his horses and bullocks, and more especially of his kangaroo dogs.
- The Greenlanders, “when they affirm anything with pleasure, suck down
- air with a certain sound;”[819] and this may be an imitation of the act
- of swallowing savoury food.
- Laughter is suppressed by the firm contraction of the orbicular muscles
- of the mouth, which prevents the great zygomatic and other muscles from
- drawing the lips backwards and upwards. The lower lip is also sometimes
- held by the teeth, and this gives a roguish expression to the face, as
- was observed with the blind and deaf Laura Bridgman.[820] The great
- zygomatic muscle is sometimes variable in its course, and I have seen a
- young woman in whom the _depressores anguli oris_ were brought into
- strong action in suppressing a smile; but this by no means gave to her
- countenance a melancholy expression, owing to the brightness of her
- eyes.
- Laughter is frequently employed in a forced manner to conceal or mask
- some other state of mind, even anger. We often see persons laughing in
- order to conceal their shame or shyness. When a person purses up his
- mouth, as if to prevent the possibility of a smile, though there is
- nothing to excite one, or nothing to prevent its free indulgence, an
- affected, solemn, or pedantic expression is given; but of such hybrid
- expressions nothing more need here be said. In the case of derision, a
- real or pretended smile or laugh is often blended with the expression
- proper to contempt, and this may pass into angry contempt or scorn. In
- such cases the meaning of the laugh or smile is to show the offending
- person that he excites only amusement.
- _Love, tender feelings, &c_.—Although the emotion of love, for instance
- that of a mother for her infant, is one of the strongest of which the
- mind is capable, it can hardly be said to have any proper or peculiar
- means of expression; and this is intelligible, as it has not habitually
- led to any special line of action. No doubt, as affection is a
- pleasurable sensation, it generally causes a gentle smile and some
- brightening of the eyes. A strong desire to touch the beloved person is
- commonly felt; and love is expressed by this means more plainly than by
- any other.[821] Hence we long to clasp in our arms those whom we
- tenderly love. We probably owe this desire to inherited habit, in
- association with the nursing and tending of our children, and with the
- mutual caresses of lovers.
- With the lower animals we see the same principle of pleasure derived
- from contact in association with love. Dogs and cats manifestly take
- pleasure in rubbing against their masters and mistresses, and in being
- rubbed or patted by them. Many kinds of monkeys, as I am assured by the
- keepers in the Zoological Gardens, delight in fondling and being
- fondled by each other, and by persons to whom they are attached. Mr.
- Bartlett has described to me the behaviour of two chimpanzees, rather
- older animals than those generally imported into this country, when
- they were first brought together. They sat opposite, touching each
- other with their much protruded lips; and the one put his hand on the
- shoulder of the other. They then mutually folded each other in their
- arms. Afterwards they stood up, each with one arm on the shoulder of
- the other, lifted up their heads, opened their mouths, and yelled with
- delight.[822]
- We Europeans are so accustomed to kissing as a mark of affection, that
- it might be thought to be innate in mankind; but this is not the case.
- Steele was mistaken when he said “Nature was its author, and it began
- with the first courtship.” Jemmy Button, the Fuegian, told me that this
- practice was unknown in his land. It is equally unknown with the New
- Zealanders, Tahitians, Papuans, Australians, Somals of Africa, and the
- Esquimaux. But it is so far innate or natural that it apparently
- depends on pleasure from close contact with a beloved person; and it is
- replaced in various parts of the world, by the rubbing of noses, as
- with the New Zealanders and Laplanders, by the rubbing or patting of
- the arms, breasts, or stomachs, or by one man striking his own face
- with the hands or feet of another. Perhaps the practice of blowing, as
- a mark of affection, on various parts of the body may depend on the
- same principle.[823]
- The feelings which are called tender are difficult to analyse; they
- seem to be compounded of affection, joy, and especially of sympathy.
- These feelings are in themselves of a pleasurable nature, excepting
- when pity is too deep, or horror is aroused, as in hearing of a
- tortured man or animal. They are remarkable under our present point of
- view from so readily exciting the secretion of tears. Many a father and
- son have wept on meeting after a long separation, especially if the
- meeting has been unexpected. No doubt extreme joy by itself tends to
- act on the lacrymal glands; but on such occasions as the foregoing
- vague thoughts of the grief which would have been felt had the father
- and son never met, will probably have passed through their minds; and
- grief naturally leads to the secretion of tears. Thus on the return of
- Ulysses:—
- “Telemachus Rose, and clung weeping round his father’s breast.
- There the pent grief rained o’er them, yearning thus.
- * * * * * *
- Thus piteously they wailed in sore unrest,
- And on their weepings had gone down the day,
- But that at last Telemachus found words to say.”
- _Worsley’s Translation of the Odyssey_, Book xvi. st. 27.
- So again when Penelope at last recognized her husband:—
- “Then from her eyelids the quick tears did start
- And she ran to him from her place, and threw
- Her arms about his neck, and a warm dew
- Of kisses poured upon him, and thus spake:”
- —Book xxiii. st. 27.
- The vivid recollection of our former home, or of long-past happy days,
- readily causes the eyes to be suffused with tears; but here, again, the
- thought naturally occurs that these days will never return. In such
- cases we may be said to sympathize with ourselves in our present, in
- comparison with our former, state. Sympathy with the distresses of
- others, even with the imaginary distresses of a heroine in a pathetic
- story, for whom we feel no affection, readily excites tears. So does
- sympathy with the happiness of others, as with that of a lover, at last
- successful after many hard trials in a well-told tale.
- Sympathy appears to constitute a separate or distinct emotion; and it
- is especially apt to excite the lacrymal glands. This holds good
- whether we give or receive sympathy. Every one must have noticed how
- readily children burst out crying if we pity them for some small hurt.
- With the melancholic insane, as Dr. Crichton Browne informs me, a kind
- word will often plunge them into unrestrained weeping. As soon as we
- express our pity for the grief of a friend, tears often come into our
- own eyes. The feeling of sympathy is commonly explained by assuming
- that, when we see or hear of suffering in another, the idea of
- suffering is called up so vividly in our own minds that we ourselves
- suffer. But this explanation is hardly sufficient, for it does not
- account for the intimate alliance between sympathy and affection. We
- undoubtedly sympathize far more deeply with a beloved than with an
- indifferent person; and the sympathy of the one gives us far more
- relief than that of the other. Yet assuredly we can sympathize with
- those for whom we feel no affection.
- Why suffering, when actually experienced by ourselves, excites weeping,
- has been discussed in a former chapter. With respect to joy, its
- natural and universal expression is laughter; and with all the races of
- man loud laughter leads to the secretion of tears more freely than does
- any other cause excepting distress. The suffusion of the eyes with
- tears, which undoubtedly occurs under great joy, though there is no
- laughter, can, as it seems to me, be explained through habit and
- association on the same principles as the effusion of tears from grief,
- although there is no screaming. Nevertheless it is not a little
- remarkable that sympathy with the distresses of others should excite
- tears more freely than our own distress; and this certainly is the
- case. Many a man, from whose eyes no suffering of his own could wring a
- tear, has shed tears at the sufferings of a beloved friend. It is still
- more remarkable that sympathy with the happiness or good fortune of
- those whom we tenderly love should lead to the same result, whilst a
- similar happiness felt by ourselves would leave our eyes dry. We
- should, however, bear in mind that the long-continued habit of
- restraint which is so powerful in checking the free flow of tears from
- bodily pain, has not been brought into play in preventing a moderate
- effusion of tears in sympathy with the sufferings or happiness of
- others.
- Music has a wonderful power, as I have elsewhere attempted to
- show,[824] of recalling in a vague and indefinite manner, those strong
- emotions which were felt during long-past ages, when, as is probable,
- our early progenitors courted each other by the aid of vocal tones. And
- as several of our strongest emotions—grief, great joy, love, and
- sympathy—lead to the free secretion of tears, it is not surprising that
- music should be apt to cause our eyes to become suffused with tears,
- especially when we are already softened by any of the tenderer
- feelings. Music often produces another peculiar effect. We know that
- every strong sensation, emotion, or excitement—extreme pain, rage,
- terror, joy, or the passion of love—all have a special tendency to
- cause the muscles to tremble; and the thrill or slight shiver which
- runs down the backbone and limbs of many persons when they are
- powerfully affected by music, seems to bear the same relation to the
- above trembling of the body, as a slight suffusion of tears from the
- power of music does to weeping from any strong and real emotion.
- _Devotion_.—As devotion is, in some degree, related to affection,
- though mainly consisting of reverence, often combined with fear, the
- expression of this state of mind may here be briefly noticed. With some
- sects, both past and present, religion and love have been strangely
- combined; and it has even been maintained, lamentable as the fact may
- be, that the holy kiss of love differs but little from that which a man
- bestows on a woman, or a woman on a man.[825] Devotion is chiefly
- expressed by the face being directed towards the heavens, with the
- eyeballs upturned. Sir C. Bell remarks that, at the approach of sleep,
- or of a fainting-fit, or of death, the pupils are drawn upwards and
- inwards; and he believes that “when we are wrapt in devotional
- feelings, and outward impressions are unheeded, the eyes are raised by
- an action neither taught nor acquired.” and that this is due to the
- same cause as in the above cases.[826] That the eyes are upturned
- during sleep is, as I hear from Professor Donders, certain. With
- babies, whilst sucking their mother’s breast, this movement of the
- eyeballs often gives to them an absurd appearance of ecstatic delight;
- and here it may be clearly perceived that a struggle is going on
- against the position naturally assumed during sleep. But Sir C. Bell’s
- explanation of the fact, which rests on the assumption that certain
- muscles are more under the control of the will than others is, as I
- hear from Professor Donders, incorrect. As the eyes are often turned up
- in prayer, without the mind being so much absorbed in thought as to
- approach to the unconsciousness of sleep, the movement is probably a
- conventional one—the result of the common belief that Heaven, the
- source of Divine power to which we pray, is seated above us.
- A humble kneeling posture, with the hands upturned and palms joined,
- appears to us, from long habit, a gesture so appropriate to devotion,
- that it might be thought to be innate; but I have not met with any
- evidence to this effect with the various extra-European races of
- mankind. During the classical period of Roman history it does not
- appear, as I hear from an excellent classic, that the hands were thus
- joined during prayer. Mr. Rensleigh Wedgwood has apparently given[827]
- the true explanation, though this implies that the attitude is one of
- slavish subjection. “When the suppliant kneels and holds up his hands
- with the palms joined, he represents a captive who proves the
- completeness of his submission by offering up his hands to be bound by
- the victor. It is the pictorial representation of the Latin _dare
- manus_, to signify submission.” Hence it is not probable that either
- the uplifting of the eyes or the joining of the open hands, under the
- influence of devotional feelings, are innate or truly expressive
- actions; and this could hardly have been expected, for it is very
- doubtful whether feelings, such as we should now rank as devotional,
- affected the hearts of men, whilst they remained during past ages in an
- uncivilized condition.
- CHAPTER IX. REFLECTION—MEDITATION-ILL-TEMPER—SULKINESS—DETERMINATION.
- The act of frowning—Reflection with an effort, or with the perception
- of something difficult or disagreeable—Abstracted
- meditation—Ill-temper—Moroseness—Obstinacy Sulkiness and
- pouting—Decision or determination—The firm closure of the mouth.
- The corrugators, by their contraction, lower the eyebrows and bring
- them together, producing vertical furrows on the forehead—that is, a
- frown. Sir C. Bell, who erroneously thought that the corrugator was
- peculiar to man, ranks it as “the most remarkable muscle of the human
- face. It knits the eyebrows with an energetic effort, which
- unaccountably, but irresistibly, conveys the idea of mind.” Or, as he
- elsewhere says, “when the eyebrows are knit, energy of mind is
- apparent, and there is the mingling of thought and emotion with the
- savage and brutal rage of the mere animal.”[901] There is much truth in
- these remarks, but hardly the whole truth. Dr. Duchenne has called the
- corrugator the muscle of reflection;[902] but this name, without some
- limitation, cannot be considered as quite correct.
- A man may be absorbed in the deepest thought, and his brow will remain
- smooth until he encounters some obstacle in his train of reasoning, or
- is interrupted by some disturbance, and then a frown passes like a
- shadow over his brow. A half-starved man may think intently how to
- obtain food, but he probably will not frown unless he encounters either
- in thought or action some difficulty, or finds the food when obtained
- nauseous. I have noticed that almost everyone instantly frowns if he
- perceives a strange or bad taste in what he is eating. I asked several
- persons, without explaining my object, to listen intently to a very
- gentle tapping sound, the nature and source of which they all perfectly
- knew, and not one frowned; but a man who joined us, and who could not
- conceive what we were all doing in profound silence, when asked to
- listen, frowned much, though not in an ill-temper, and said he could
- not in the least understand what we all wanted. Dr. Piderit[903] who
- has published remarks to the same effect, adds that stammerers
- generally frown in speaking, and that a man in doing even so trifling a
- thing as pulling on a boot, frowns if he finds it too tight. Some
- persons are such habitual frowners, that the mere effort of speaking
- almost always causes their brows to contract.
- Men of all races frown when they are in any way perplexed in thought,
- as I infer from the answers which I have received to my queries; but I
- framed them badly, confounding absorbed meditation with perplexed
- reflection. Nevertheless, it is clear that the Australians, Malays,
- Hindoos, and Kafirs of South Africa frown, when they are puzzled.
- Dobritzhoffer remarks that the Guaranies of South America on like
- occasions knit their brows.[904]
- From these considerations, we may conclude that frowning is not the
- expression of simple reflection, however profound, or of attention,
- however close, but of something difficult or displeasing encountered in
- a train of thought or in action. Deep reflection can, however, seldom
- be long carried on without some difficulty, so that it will generally
- be accompanied by a frown. Hence it is that frowning commonly gives to
- the countenance, as Sir C. Bell remarks, an aspect of intellectual
- energy. But in order that this effect may be produced, the eyes must be
- clear and steady, or they may be cast downwards, as often occurs in
- deep thought. The countenance must not be otherwise disturbed, as in
- the case of an ill-tempered or peevish man, or of one who shows the
- effects of prolonged suffering, with dulled eyes and drooping jaw, or
- who perceives a bad taste in his food, or who finds it difficult to
- perform some trifling act, such as threading a needle. In these cases a
- frown may often be seen, but it will be accompanied by some other
- expression, which will entirely prevent the countenance having an
- appearance of intellectual energy or of profound thought.
- We may now inquire how it is that a frown should express the perception
- of something difficult or disagreeable, either in thought or action. In
- the same way as naturalists find it advisable to trace the
- embryological development of an organ in order fully to understand its
- structure, so with the movements of expression it is advisable to
- follow as nearly as possible the same plan. The earliest and almost
- sole expression seen during the first days of infancy, and then often
- exhibited is that displayed during the act of screaming; and screaming
- is excited, both at first and for some time afterwards, by every
- distressing or displeasing sensation and emotion,—by hunger, pain,
- anger, jealousy, fear, &c. At such times the muscles round the eyes are
- strongly contracted; and this, as I believe, explains to a large extent
- the act of frowning during the remainder of our lives. I repeatedly
- observed my own infants, from under the age of one week to that of two
- or three months, and found that when a screaming-fit came on gradually,
- the first sign was the contraction of the corrugators, which produced a
- slight frown, quickly followed by the contraction of the other muscles
- round the eyes. When an infant is uncomfortable or unwell, little
- frowns—as I record in my notes—may be seen incessantly passing like
- shadows over its face; these being generally, but not always, followed
- sooner or later by a crying-fit. For instance, I watched for some time
- a baby, between seven and eight weeks old, sucking some milk which was
- cold, and therefore displeasing to him; and a steady little frown was
- maintained all the time. This was never developed into an actual
- crying-fit, though occasionally every stage of close approach could be
- observed.
- As the habit of contracting the brows has been followed by infants
- during innumerable generations, at the commencement of every crying or
- screaming fit, it has become firmly associated with the incipient sense
- of something distressing or disagreeable. Hence under similar
- circumstances it would be apt to be continued during maturity, although
- never then developed into a crying-fit. Screaming or weeping begins to
- be voluntarily restrained at an early period of life, whereas frowning
- is hardly ever restrained at any age. It is perhaps worth notice that
- with children much given to weeping, anything which perplexes their
- minds, and which would cause most other children merely to frown,
- readily makes them weep. So with certain classes of the insane, any
- effort of mind, however slight, which with an habitual frowner would
- cause a slight frown, leads to their weeping in an unrestrained manner.
- It is not more surprising that the habit of contracting the brows at
- the first perception of something distressing, although gained during
- infancy, should be retained during the rest of our lives, than that
- many other associated habits acquired at an early age should be
- permanently retained both by man and the lower animals. For instance,
- full-grown cats, when feeling warm and comfortable, often retain the
- habit of alternately protruding their fore-feet with extended toes,
- which habit they practised for a definite purpose whilst sucking their
- mothers.
- Another and distinct cause has probably strengthened the habit of
- frowning, whenever the mind is intent on any subject and encounters
- some difficulty. Vision is the most important of all the senses, and
- during primeval times the closest attention must have been incessantly:
- directed towards distant objects for the sake of obtaining prey and
- avoiding danger. I remember being struck, whilst travelling in parts of
- South America, which were dangerous from the presence of Indians, how
- incessantly, yet as it appeared unconsciously, the half-wild Gauchos
- closely scanned the whole horizon. Now, when any one with no covering
- on his head (as must have been aboriginally the case with mankind),
- strives to the utmost to distinguish in broad daylight, and especially
- if the sky is bright, a distant object, he almost invariably contracts
- his brows to prevent the entrance of too much light; the lower eyelids,
- cheeks, and upper lip being at the same time raised, so as to lessen
- the orifice of the eyes. I have purposely asked several persons, young
- and old, to look, under the above circumstances, at distant objects,
- making them believe that I only wished to test the power of their
- vision; and they all behaved in the manner just described. Some of
- them, also, put their open, flat hands over their eyes to keep out the
- excess of light. Gratiolet, after making some remarks to nearly the
- same effect,[905] says, “Ce sont là des attitudes de vision difficile.”
- He concludes that the muscles round the eyes contract partly for the
- sake of excluding too much light (which appears to me the more
- important end), and partly to prevent all rays striking the retina,
- except those which come direct from the object that is scrutinized. Mr.
- Bowman, whom I consulted on this point, thinks that the contraction of
- the surrounding muscles may, in addition, “partly sustain the
- consensual movements of the two eyes, by giving a firmer support while
- the globes are brought to binocular vision by their own proper
- muscles.”
- As the effort of viewing with care under a bright light a distant
- object is both difficult and irksome, and as this effort has been
- habitually accompanied, during numberless generations, by the
- contraction of the eyebrows, the habit of frowning will thus have been
- much strengthened; although it was originally practised during infancy
- from a quite independent cause, namely as the first step in the
- protection of the eyes during screaming. There is, indeed, much
- analogy, as far as the state of the mind is concerned, between intently
- scrutinizing a distant object, and following out an obscure train of
- thought, or performing some little and troublesome mechanical work. The
- belief that the habit of contracting the brows is continued when there
- is no need whatever to exclude too much light, receives support from
- the cases formerly alluded to, in which the eyebrows or eyelids are
- acted on under certain circumstances in a useless manner, from having
- been similarly used, under analogous circumstances, for a serviceable
- purpose. For instance, we voluntarily close our eyes when we do not
- wish to see any object, and we are apt to close them, when we reject a
- proposition, as if we could not or would not see it; or when we think
- about something horrible. We raise our eyebrows when we wish to see
- quickly all round us, and we often do the same, when we earnestly
- desire to remember something; acting as if we endeavoured to see it.
- _Abstraction. Meditation_.—When a person is lost in thought with his
- mind absent, or, as it is sometimes said, “when he is in a brown
- study,” he does not frown, but his eyes appear vacant. The lower
- eyelids are generally raised and wrinkled, in the same manner as when a
- short-sighted person tries to distinguish a distant object; and the
- upper orbicular muscles are at the same time slightly contracted. The
- wrinkling of the lower eyelids under these circumstances has been
- observed with some savages, as by Mr. Dyson Lacy with the Australians
- of Queensland, and several times by Mr. Geach with the Malays of the
- interior of Malacca. What the meaning or cause of this action may be,
- cannot at present be explained; but here we have another instance of
- movement round the eyes in relation to the state of the mind.
- The vacant expression of the eyes is very peculiar, and at once shows
- when a man is completely lost in thought. Professor Donders has, with
- his usual kindness, investigated this subject for me. He has observed
- others in this condition, and has been himself observed by Professor
- Engelmann. The eyes are not then fixed on any object, and therefore
- not, as I had imagined, on some distant object. The lines of vision of
- the two eyes even often become slightly divergent; the divergence, if
- the head be held vertically, with the plane of vision horizontal,
- amounting to an angle of 2° as a maximum. This was ascertained by
- observing the crossed double image of a distant object. When the head
- droops forward, as often occurs with a man absorbed in thought, owing
- to the general relaxation of his muscles, if the plane of vision be
- still horizontal, the eyes are necessarily a little turned upwards, and
- then the divergence is as much as 3°, or 3° 5’: if the eyes are turned
- still more upwards, it amounts to between 6° and 7°. Professor Donders
- attributes this divergence to the almost complete relaxation of certain
- muscles of the eyes, which would be apt to follow from the mind being
- wholly absorbed.[906] The active condition of the muscles of the eyes
- is that of convergence; and Professor Donders remarks, as bearing on
- their divergence during a period of complete abstraction, that when one
- eye becomes blind, it almost always, after a short lapse of time,
- deviates outwards; for its muscles are no longer used in moving the
- eyeball inwards for the sake of binocular vision.
- Perplexed reflection is often accompanied by certain movements or
- gestures. At such times we commonly raise our hands to our foreheads,
- mouths, or chins; but we do not act thus, as far as I have seen, when
- we are quite lost in meditation, and no difficulty is encountered.
- Plautus, describing in one of his plays[907] a puzzled man, says, “Now
- look, he has pillared his chin upon his hand.” Even so trifling and
- apparently unmeaning a gesture as the raising of the hand to the face
- has been observed with some savages. Mr. J. Mansel Weale has seen it
- with the Kafirs of South Africa; and the native chief Gaika adds, that
- men then “sometimes pull their beards.” Mr. Washington Matthews, who
- attended to some of the wildest tribes of Indians in the western
- regions of the United States, remarks that he has seen them when
- concentrating their thoughts, bring their “hands, usually the thumb and
- index finger, in contact with some part of the face, commonly the upper
- lip.” We can understand why the forehead should be pressed or rubbed,
- as deep thought tries the brain; but why the hand should be raised to
- the mouth or face is far from clear.
- _Ill-temper_.—We have seen that frowning is the natural expression of
- some difficulty encountered, or of something disagreeable experienced
- either in thought or action, and he whose mind is often and readily
- affected in this way, will be apt to be ill-tempered, or slightly
- angry, or peevish, and will commonly show it by frowning. But a cross
- expression, due to a frown, may be counteracted, if the mouth appears
- sweet, from being habitually drawn into a smile, and the eyes are
- bright and cheerful. So it will be if the eye is clear and steady, and
- there is the appearance of earnest reflection. Frowning, with some
- depression of the corners of the mouth, which is a sign of grief, gives
- an air of peevishness. If a child (see Plate IV., fig. 2)[908] frowns
- much whilst crying, but does not strongly contract in the usual manner
- the orbicular muscles, a well-marked expression of anger or even of
- rage, together with misery, is displayed.
- Ill-temper. Plate IV
- If the whole frowning brow be drawn much downward by the contraction of
- the pyramidal muscles of the nose, which produces transverse wrinkles
- or folds across the base of the nose, the expression becomes one of
- moroseness. Duchenne believes that the contraction of this muscle,
- without any frowning, gives the appearance of extreme and aggressive
- hardness.[909] But I much doubt whether this is a true or natural
- expression. I have shown Duchenne’s photograph of a young man, with
- this muscle strongly contracted by means of galvanism, to eleven
- persons, including some artists, and none of them could form an idea
- what was intended, except one, a girl, who answered correctly, “surely
- reserve.” When I first looked at this photograph, knowing what was
- intended, my imagination added, as I believe, what was necessary,
- namely, a frowning brow; and consequently the expression appeared to me
- true and extremely morose.
- A firmly closed mouth, in addition to a lowered and frowning brow,
- gives determination to the expression, or may make it obstinate and
- sullen. How it comes that the firm closure of the mouth gives the
- appearance of determination will presently be discussed. An expression
- of sullen obstinacy has been clearly recognized by my informants, in
- the natives of six different regions of Australia. It is well marked,
- according to Mr. Scott, with the Hindoos. It has been recognized with
- the Malays, Chinese, Kafirs, Abyssinians, and in a conspicuous degree,
- according to Dr. Rothrock, with the wild Indians of North America, and
- according to Mr. D. Forbes, with the Aymaras of Bolivia. I have also
- observed it with the Araucanos of southern Chili. Mr. Dyson Lacy
- remarks that the natives of Australia, when in this frame of mind,
- sometimes fold their arms across their breasts, an attitude which may
- be seen with us. A firm determination, amounting to obstinacy, is,
- also, sometimes expressed by both shoulders being kept raised, the
- meaning of which gesture will be explained in the following chapter.
- With young children sulkiness is shown by pouting, or, as it is
- sometimes called, “making a snout.”[910] When the corners of the mouth
- are much depressed, the lower lip is a little everted and protruded;
- and this is likewise called a pout. But the pouting here referred to,
- consists of the protrusion of both lips into a tubular form, sometimes
- to such an extent as to project as far as the end of the nose, if this
- be short. Pouting is generally accompanied by frowning, and sometimes
- by the utterance of a booing or whooing noise. This expression is
- remarkable, as almost the sole one, as far as I know, which is
- exhibited much more plainly during childhood, at least with Europeans,
- than during maturity. There is, however, some tendency to the
- protrusion of the lips with the adults of all races under the influence
- of great rage. Some children pout when they are shy, and they can then
- hardly be called sulky.
- From inquiries which I have made in several large families, pouting
- does not seem very common with European children; but it prevails
- throughout the world, and must be both common and strongly marked with
- most savage races, as it has caught the attention of many observers. It
- has been noticed in eight different districts of Australia; and one of
- my informants remarks how greatly the lips of the children are then
- protruded. Two observers have seen pouting with the children of
- Hindoos; three, with those of the Kafirs and Fingoes of South Africa,
- and with the Hottentots; and two, with the children of the wild Indians
- of North America. Pouting has also been observed with the Chinese,
- Abyssinians, Malays of Malacca, Dyaks of Borneo, and often with the New
- Zealanders. Mr. Mansel Weale informs me that he has seen the lips much
- protruded, not only with the children of the Kafirs, but with the
- adults of both sexes when sulky; and Mr. Stack has sometimes observed
- the same thing with the men, and very frequently with the women of New
- Zealand. A trace of the same expression may occasionally be detected
- even with adult Europeans.
- We thus see that the protrusion of the lips, especially with young
- children, is characteristic of sulkiness throughout the greater part of
- the world. This movement apparently results from the retention, chiefly
- during youth, of a primordial habit, or from an occasional reversion to
- it. Young orangs and chimpanzees protrude their lips to an
- extraordinary degree, as described in a former chapter, when they are
- discontented, somewhat angry, or sulky; also when they are surprised, a
- little frightened, and even when slightly pleased. Their mouths are
- protruded apparently for the sake of making the various noises proper
- to these several states of mind; and its shape, as I observed with the
- chimpanzee, differed slightly when the cry of pleasure and that of
- anger were uttered. As soon as these animals become enraged, the shape
- of the month wholly changes, and the teeth are exposed. The adult orang
- when wounded is said to emit “a singular cry, consisting at first of
- high notes, which at length deepen into a low roar. While giving out
- the high notes he thrusts out his lips into a funnel shape, but in
- uttering the low notes he holds his mouth wide open.”[911] With the
- gorilla, the lower lip is said to be capable of great elongation. If
- then our semi-human progenitors protruded their lips when sulky or a
- little angered, in the same manner as do the existing anthropoid apes,
- it is not an anomalous, though a curious fact, that our children should
- exhibit, when similarly affected, a trace of the same expression,
- together with some tendency to utter a noise. For it is not at all
- unusual for animals to retain, more or less perfectly, during early
- youth, and subsequently to lose, characters which were aboriginally
- possessed by their adult progenitors, and which are still retained by
- distinct species, their near relations.
- Nor is it an anomalous fact that the children of savages should exhibit
- a stronger tendency to protrude their lips, when sulky, than the
- children of civilized Europeans; for the essence of savagery seems to
- consist in the retention of a primordial condition, and this
- occasionally holds good even with bodily peculiarities.[912] It may be
- objected to this view of the origin of pouting, that the anthropoid
- apes likewise protrude their lips when astonished and even when a
- little pleased; whilst with us this expression is generally confined to
- a sulky frame of mind. But we shall see in a future chapter that with
- men of various races surprise does sometimes lead to a slight
- protrusion of the lips, though great surprise or astonishment is more
- commonly shown by the mouth being widely opened. As when we smile or
- laugh we draw back the corners of the mouth, we have lost any tendency
- to protrude the lips, when pleased, if indeed our early progenitors
- thus expressed pleasure.
- A little gesture made by sulky children may here be noticed, namely,
- their “showing a cold shoulder.” This has a different meaning, as, I
- believe, from the keeping both shoulders raised. A cross child, sitting
- on its parent’s knee, will lift up the near shoulder, then jerk it
- away, as if from a caress, and afterwards give a backward push with it,
- as if to push away the offender. I have seen a child, standing at some
- distance from any one, clearly express its feelings by raising one
- shoulder, giving it a little backward movement, and then turning away
- its whole body.
- _Decision or determination_.—The firm closure of the mouth tends to
- give an expression of determination or decision to the countenance. No
- determined man probably ever had an habitually gaping mouth. Hence,
- also, a small and weak lower jaw, which seems to indicate that the
- mouth is not habitually and firmly closed, is commonly thought to be
- characteristic of feebleness of character. A prolonged effort of any
- kind, whether of body or mind, implies previous determination; and if
- it can be shown that the mouth is generally closed with firmness before
- and during a great and continued exertion of the muscular system, then,
- through the principle of association, the mouth would almost certainly
- be closed as soon as any determined resolution was taken. Now several
- observers have noticed that a man, in commencing any violent muscular
- effort, invariably first distends his lungs with air, and then
- compresses it by the strong contraction of the muscles of the chest;
- and to effect this the mouth must be firmly closed. Moreover, as soon
- as the man is compelled to draw breath, he still keeps his chest as
- much distended as possible.
- Various causes have been assigned for this manner of acting. Sir C.
- Bell maintains[913] that the chest is distended with air, and is kept
- distended at such times, in order to give a fixed support to the
- muscles which are thereto attached. Hence, as he remarks, when two men
- are engaged in a deadly contest, a terrible silence prevails, broken
- only by hard stifled breathing. There is silence, because to expel the
- air in the utterance of any sound would be to relax the support for the
- muscles of the arms. If an outcry is heard, supposing the struggle to
- take place in the dark, we at once know that one of the two has given
- up in despair.
- Gratiolet admits[914] that when a man has to struggle with another to
- his utmost, or has to support a great weight, or to keep for a long
- time the same forced attitude, it is necessary for him first to make a
- deep inspiration, and then to cease breathing; but he thinks that Sir
- C. Bell’s explanation is erroneous. He maintains that arrested
- respiration retards the circulation of the blood, of which I believe
- there is no doubt, and he adduces some curious evidence from the
- structure of the lower animals, showing, on the one hand, that a
- retarded circulation is necessary for prolonged muscular exertion, and,
- on the other hand, that a rapid circulation is necessary for rapid
- movements. According to this view, when we commence any great exertion,
- we close our mouths and stop breathing, in order to retard the
- circulation of the blood. Gratiolet sums up the subject by saying,
- “C’est là la vraie théorie de l’effort continu;” but how far this
- theory is admitted by other physiologists I do not know.
- Dr. Piderit accounts[915] for the firm closure of the mouth during
- strong muscular exertion, on the principle that the influence of the
- will spreads to other muscles besides those necessarily brought into
- action in making any particular exertion; and it is natural that the
- muscles of respiration and of the mouth, from being so habitually used,
- should be especially liable to be thus acted on. It appears to me that
- there probably is some truth in this view, for we are apt to press the
- teeth hard together during violent exertion, and this is not requisite
- to prevent expiration, whilst the muscles of the chest are strongly
- contracted.
- Lastly, when a man has to perform some delicate and difficult
- operation, not requiring the exertion of any strength, he nevertheless
- generally closes his mouth and ceases for a time to breathe; but he
- acts thus in order that the movements of his chest may not disturb,
- those of his arms. A person, for instance, whilst threading a needle,
- may be seen to compress his lips and either to stop breathing, or to
- breathe as quietly as possible. So it was, as formerly stated, with a
- young and sick chimpanzee, whilst it amused itself by killing flies
- with its knuckles, as they buzzed about on the window-panes. To perform
- an action, however trifling, if difficult, implies some amount of
- previous determination.
- There appears nothing improbable in all the above assigned causes
- having come into play in different degrees, either conjointly or
- separately, on various occasions. The result would be a
- well-established habit, now perhaps inherited, of firmly closing the
- mouth at the commencement of and during any violent and prolonged
- exertion, or any delicate operation. Through the principle of
- association there would also be a strong tendency towards this same
- habit, as soon as the mind had resolved on any particular action or
- line of conduct, even before there was any bodily exertion, or if none
- were requisite. The habitual and firm closure of the mouth would thus
- come to show decision of character; and decision readily passes into
- obstinacy.
- CHAPTER X. HATRED AND ANGER.
- Hatred—Rage, effects of on the system—Uncovering of the teeth—Rage in
- the insane—Anger and indignation—As expressed by the various races of
- man—Sneering and defiance—The uncovering of the canine tooth on one
- side of the face.
- If we have suffered or expect to suffer some wilful injury from a man,
- or if he is in any way offensive to us, we dislike him; and dislike
- easily rises into hatred. Such feelings, if experienced in a moderate
- degree, are not clearly expressed by any movement of the body or
- features, excepting perhaps by a certain gravity of behaviour, or by
- some ill-temper. Few individuals, however, can long reflect about a
- hated person, without feeling and exhibiting signs of indignation or
- rage. But if the offending person be quite insignificant, we experience
- merely disdain or contempt. If, on the other hand, he is all-powerful,
- then hatred passes into terror, as when a slave thinks about a cruel
- master, or a savage about a bloodthirsty malignant deity.[1001] Most of
- our emotions are so closely connected with their expression, that they
- hardly exist if the body remains passive—the nature of the expression
- depending in chief part on the nature of the actions which have been
- habitually performed under this particular state of the mind. A man,
- for instance, may know that his life is in the extremest peril, and may
- strongly desire to save if; yet, as Louis XVI. said, when surrounded by
- a fierce mob, “Am I afraid? feel my pulse.” So a man may intensely hate
- another, but until his bodily frame is affected, he cannot be said to
- be enraged.
- _Rage_.—I have already had occasion to treat of this emotion in the
- third chapter, when discussing the direct influence of the excited
- sensorium on the body, in combination with the effects of habitually
- associated actions. Rage exhibits itself in the most diversified
- manner. The heart and circulation are always affected; the face reddens
- or becomes purple, with the veins on the forehead and neck distended.
- The reddening of the skin has been observed with the copper-coloured
- Indians of South America,[1002] and even, as it is said, on the white
- cicatrices left by old wounds on negroes.[1003] Monkeys also redden
- from passion. With one of my own infants, under four months old, I
- repeatedly observed that the first symptom of an approaching passion
- was the rushing of the blood into his bare scalp. On the other hand,
- the action of the heart is sometimes so much impeded by great rage,
- that the countenance becomes pallid or livid,[1004] and not a few men
- with heart-disease have dropped down dead under this powerful emotion.
- The respiration is likewise affected; the chest heaves, and the dilated
- nostrils quiver.[1005] As Tennyson writes, “sharp breaths of anger
- puffed her fairy nostrils out.” Hence we have such expressions as
- “breathing out vengeance,” and “fuming with anger.”[1006]
- The excited brain gives strength to the muscles, and at the same time
- energy to the will. The body is commonly held erect ready for instant
- action, but sometimes it is bent forward towards the offending person,
- with the limbs more or less rigid. The mouth is generally closed with
- firmness, showing fixed determination, and the teeth are clenched or
- ground together. Such gestures as the raising of the arms, with the
- fists clenched, as if to strike the offender, are common. Few men in a
- great passion, and telling some one to begone, can resist acting as if
- they intended to strike or push the man violently away. The desire,
- indeed, to strike often becomes so intolerably strong, that inanimate
- objects are struck or dashed to the ground; but the gestures frequently
- become altogether purposeless or frantic. Young children, when in a
- violent rage roll on the ground on their backs or bellies, screaming,
- kicking, scratching, or biting everything within reach. So it is, as I
- hear from Mr. Scott, with Hindoo children; and, as we have seen, with
- the young of the anthropomorphous apes.
- But the muscular system is often affected in a wholly different way;
- for trembling is a frequent consequence of extreme rage. The paralysed
- lips then refuse to obey the will, “and the voice sticks in the
- throat;”[1007] or it is rendered loud, harsh, and discordant. If there
- be much and rapid speaking, the mouth froths. The hair sometimes
- bristles; but I shall return to this subject in another chapter, when I
- treat of the mingled emotions of rage and terror. There is in most
- cases a strongly-marked frown on the forehead; for this follows from
- the sense of anything displeasing or difficult, together with
- concentration of mind. But sometimes the brow, instead of being much
- contracted and lowered, remains smooth, with the glaring eyes kept
- widely open. The eyes are always bright, or may, as Homer expresses it,
- glisten with fire. They are sometimes bloodshot, and are said to
- protrude from their sockets—the result, no doubt, of the head being
- gorged with blood, as shown by the veins being distended. According to
- Gratiolet, “the pupils are always contracted in rage,” and I hear from
- Dr. Crichton Browne that this is the case in the fierce delirium of
- meningitis; but the movements of the iris under the influence of the
- different emotions is a very obscure subject.[1008]
- Shakspeare sums up the chief characteristics of rage as follows:—
- “In peace there’s nothing so becomes a man,
- As modest stillness and humility;
- But when the blast of war blows in our ears,
- Then imitate the action of the tiger:
- Stiffen the sinews, summon up the blood,
- Then lend the eye a terrible aspect;
- Now set the teeth, and stretch the nostril wide,
- Hold hard the breath, and bend up every spirit
- To his full height! On, on, you noblest English.”
- _Henry V_., act iii. sc. 1.
- The lips are sometimes protruded during rage in a manner, the meaning
- of which I do not understand, unless it depends on our descent from
- some ape-like animal. Instances have been observed, not only with
- Europeans, but with the Australians and Hindoos. The lips, however, are
- much more commonly retracted, the grinning or clenched teeth being thus
- exposed. This has been noticed by almost every one who has written on
- expression.[1009] The appearance is as if the teeth were uncovered,
- ready for seizing or tearing an enemy, though there may be no intention
- of acting in this manner. Mr. Dyson Lacy has seen this grinning
- expression with the Australians, when quarrelling, and so has Gaika
- with the Kafirs of South America. Dickens,[1010] in speaking of an
- atrocious murderer who had just been caught, and was surrounded by a
- furious mob, describes “the people as jumping up one behind another,
- snarling with their teeth, and making at him like wild beasts.” Every
- one who has had much to do with young children must have seen how
- naturally they take to biting, when in a passion. It seems as
- instinctive in them as in young crocodiles, who snap their little jaws
- as soon as they emerge from the egg.
- A grinning expression and the protrusion of the lips appear sometimes
- to go together. A close observer says that he has seen many instances
- of intense hatred (which can hardly be distinguished from rage, more or
- less suppressed) in Orientals, and once in an elderly English woman. In
- all these cases there “was a grin, not a scowl—the lips lengthening,
- the cheeks settling downwards, the eyes half-closed, whilst the brow
- remained perfectly calm.”[1011]
- This retraction of the lips and uncovering of the teeth during
- paroxysms of rage, as if to bite the offender, is so remarkable,
- considering how seldom the teeth are used by men in fighting, that I
- inquired from Dr. J. Crichton Browne whether the habit was common in
- the insane whose passions are unbridled. He informs me that he has
- repeatedly observed it both with the insane and idiotic, and has given
- me the following illustrations:—
- Shortly before receiving my letter, he witnessed an uncontrollable
- outbreak of anger and delusive jealousy in an insane lady. At first she
- vituperated her husband, and whilst doing so foamed at the mouth. Next
- she approached close to him with compressed lips, and a virulent set
- frown. Then she drew back her lips, especially the corners of the upper
- lip, and showed her teeth, at the same time aiming a vicious blow at
- him. A second case is that of an old soldier, who, when he is requested
- to conform to the rules of the establishment, gives way to discontent,
- terminating in fury. He commonly begins by asking Dr. Browne whether he
- is not ashamed to treat him in such a manner. He then swears and
- blasphemes, paces tip and down, tosses his arms wildly about, and
- menaces any one near him. At last, as his exasperation culminates, he
- rushes up towards Dr. Browne with a peculiar sidelong movement, shaking
- his doubled fist, and threatening destruction. Then his upper lip may
- be seen to be raised, especially at the corners, so that his huge
- canine teeth are exhibited. He hisses forth his curses through his set
- teeth, and his whole expression assumes the character of extreme
- ferocity. A similar description is applicable to another man, excepting
- that he generally foams at the mouth and spits, dancing and jumping
- about in a strange rapid manner, shrieking out his maledictions in a
- shrill falsetto voice.
- Dr. Browne also informs me of the case of an epileptic idiot, incapable
- of independent movements, and who spends the whole day in playing with
- some toys; but his temper is morose and easily roused into fierceness.
- When any one touches his toys, he slowly raises his head from its
- habitual downward position, and fixes his eyes on the offender, with a
- tardy yet angry scowl. If the annoyance be repeated, he draws back his
- thick lips and reveals a prominent row of hideous fangs (large canines
- being especially noticeable), and then makes a quick and cruel clutch
- with his open hand at the offending person. The rapidity of this
- clutch, as Dr. Browne remarks, is marvellous in a being ordinarily so
- torpid that he takes about fifteen seconds, when attracted by any
- noise, to turn his head from one side to the other. If, when thus
- incensed, a handkerchief, book, or other article, be placed into his
- hands, he drags it to his mouth and bites it. Mr. Nicol has likewise
- described to me two cases of insane patients, whose lips are retracted
- during paroxysms of rage.
- Dr. Maudsley, after detailing various strange animal-like traits in
- idiots, asks whether these are not due to the reappearance of primitive
- instincts—“a faint echo from a far-distant past, testifying to a
- kinship which man has almost outgrown.” He adds, that as every human
- brain passes, in the course of its development, through the same stages
- as those occurring in the lower vertebrate animals, and as the brain of
- an idiot is in an arrested condition, we may presume that it “will
- manifest its most primitive functions, and no higher functions.” Dr.
- Maudsley thinks that the same view may be extended to the brain in its
- degenerated condition in some insane patients; and asks, whence come
- “the savage snarl, the destructive disposition, the obscene language,
- the wild howl, the offensive habits, displayed by some of the insane?
- Why should a human being, deprived of his reason, ever become so brutal
- in character, as some do, unless he has the brute nature within
- him?”[1012] This question must, as it would appear, he answered in the
- affirmative.
- _Anger, Indignation_.—These states of the mind differ from rage only in
- degree, and there is no marked distinction in their characteristic
- signs. Under moderate anger the action of the heart is a little
- increased, the colour heightened, and the eyes become bright. The
- respiration is likewise a little hurried; and as all the muscles
- serving for this function act in association, the wings of the nostrils
- are somewhat raised to allow of a free indraught of air; and this is a
- highly characteristic sign of indignation. The mouth is commonly
- compressed, and there is almost always a frown on the brow. Instead of
- the frantic gestures of extreme rage, an indignant man unconsciously
- throws himself into an attitude ready for attacking or striking his
- enemy, whom he will perhaps scan from head to foot in defiance. He
- carries his head erect, with his chest well expanded, and the feet
- planted firmly on the ground. He holds his arms in various positions,
- with one or both elbows squared, or with the arms rigidly suspended by
- his sides. With Europeans the fists are commonly clenched.[1013] The
- figures 1 and 2 in Plate VI. are fairly good representations of men
- simulating indignation. Any one may see in a mirror, if he will vividly
- imagine that he has been insulted and demands an explanation in an
- angry tone of voice, that he suddenly and unconsciously throws himself
- into some such attitude.
- Anger and Indignation. Plate VI
- Rage, anger, and indignation are exhibited in nearly the same manner
- throughout the world; and the following descriptions may be worth
- giving as evidence of this, and as illustrations of some of the
- foregoing remarks. There is, however, an exception with respect to
- clenching the fists, which seems confined chiefly to the men who fight
- with their fists. With the Australians only one of my informants has
- seen the fists clenched. All agree about the body being held erect; and
- all, with two exceptions, state that the brows are heavily contracted.
- Some of them allude to the firmly-compressed mouth, the distended
- nostrils, and flashing eyes. According to the Rev. Mr. Taplin, rage,
- with the Australians, is expressed by the lips being protruded, the
- eyes being widely open; and in the case of the women by their dancing
- about and casting dust into the air. Another observer speaks of the
- native men, when enraged, throwing their arms wildly about.
- I have received similar accounts, except as to the clenching of the
- fists, in regard to the Malays of the Malacca peninsula, the
- Abyssinians, and the natives of South Africa. So it is with the Dakota
- Indians of North America; and, according to Mr. Matthews, they then
- hold their heads erect, frown, and often stalk away with long strides.
- Mr. Bridges states that the Fuegians, when enraged, frequently stamp on
- the ground, walk distractedly about, sometimes cry and grow pale. The
- Rev. Mr. Stack watched a New Zealand man and woman quarrelling, and
- made the following entry in his note-book: “Eyes dilated, body swayed
- violently backwards and forwards, head inclined forwards, fists
- clenched, now thrown behind the body, now directed towards each other’s
- faces.” Mr. Swinhoe says that my description agrees with what he has
- seen of the Chinese, excepting that an angry man generally inclines his
- body towards his antagonist, and pointing at him, pours forth a volley
- of abuse.
- Lastly, with respect to the natives of India, Mr. J. Scott has sent me
- a full description of their gestures and expression when enraged. Two
- low-caste Bengalees disputed about a loan. At first they were calm, but
- soon grew furious and poured forth the grossest abuse on each other’s
- relations and progenitors for many generations past. Their gestures
- were very different from those of Europeans; for though their chests
- were expanded and shoulders squared, their arms remained rigidly
- suspended, with the elbows turned inwards and the hands alternately
- clenched and opened. Their shoulders were often raised high, and then
- again lowered. They looked fiercely at each other from under their
- lowered and strongly wrinkled brows, and their protruded lips were
- firmly closed. They approached each other, with heads and necks
- stretched forwards, and pushed, scratched, and grasped at each other.
- This protrusion of the head and body seems a common gesture with the
- enraged; and I have noticed it with degraded English women whilst
- quarrelling violently in the streets. In such cases it may be presumed
- that neither party expects to receive a blow from the other.
- A Bengalee employed in the Botanic Gardens was accused, in the presence
- of Mr. Scott, by the native overseer of having stolen a valuable plant.
- He listened silently and scornfully to the accusation; his attitude
- erect, chest expanded, mouth closed, lips protruding, eyes firmly set
- and penetrating. He then defiantly maintained his innocence, with
- upraised and clenched hands, his head being now pushed forwards, with
- the eyes widely open and eyebrows raised. Mr. Scott also watched two
- Mechis, in Sikhim, quarrelling about their share of payment. They soon
- got into a furious passion, and then their bodies became less erect,
- with their heads pushed forwards; they made grimaces at each other;
- their shoulders were raised; their arms rigidly bent inwards at the
- elbows, and their hands spasmodically closed, but not properly
- clenched. They continually approached and retreated from each other,
- and often raised their arms as if to strike, but their hands were open,
- and no blow was given. Mr. Scott made similar observations on the
- Lepchas whom he often saw quarrelling, and he noticed that they kept
- their arms rigid and almost parallel to their bodies, with the hands
- pushed somewhat backwards and partially closed, but not clenched.
- _Sneering, Defiance: Uncovering the canine tooth on one side_.—The
- expression which I wish here to consider differs but little from that
- already described, when the lips are retracted and the grinning teeth
- exposed. The difference consists solely in the upper lip being
- retracted in such a manner that the canine tooth on one side of the
- face alone is shown; the face itself being generally a little upturned
- and half averted from the person causing offence. The other signs of
- rage are not necessarily present. This expression may occasionally be
- observed in a person who sneers at or defies another, though there may
- be no real anger; as when any one is playfully accused of some fault,
- and answers, “I scorn the imputation.” The expression is not a common
- one, but I have seen it exhibited with perfect distinctness by a lady
- who was being quizzed by another person. It was described by Parsons as
- long ago as 1746, with an engraving, showing the uncovered canine on
- one side.[1014] Mr. Rejlander, without my having made any allusion to
- the subject, asked me whether I had ever noticed this expression, as he
- had been much struck by it. He has photographed for me (Plate IV. fig
- 1) a lady, who sometimes unintentionally displays the canine on one
- side, and who can do so voluntarily with unusual distinctness.
- The expression of a half-playful sneer graduates into one of great
- ferocity when, together with a heavily frowning brow and fierce eye,
- the canine tooth is exposed. A Bengalee boy was accused before Mr.
- Scott of some misdeed. The delinquent did not dare to give vent to his
- wrath in words, but it was plainly shown on his countenance, sometimes
- by a defiant frown, and sometimes “by a thoroughly canine snarl.” When
- this was exhibited, “the corner of the lip over the eye-tooth, which
- happened in this case to be large and projecting, was raised on the
- side of his accuser, a strong frown being still retained on the brow.”
- Sir C. Bell states[1015] that the actor Cooke could express the most
- determined hate “when with the oblique cast of his eyes he drew up the
- outer part of the upper lip, and discovered a sharp angular tooth.”
- The uncovering of the canine tooth is the result of a double movement.
- The angle or corner of the mouth is drawn a little backwards, and at
- the same time a muscle which runs parallel to and near the nose draws
- up the outer part of the upper lip, and exposes the canine on this side
- of the face. The contraction of this muscle makes a distinct furrow on
- the cheek, and produces strong wrinkles under the eye, especially at
- its inner corner. The action is the same as that of a snarling dog; and
- a dog when pretending to fight often draws up the lip on one side
- alone, namely that facing his antagonist. Our word _sneer_ is in fact
- the same as _snarl_, which was originally _snar_, the _l_ “being merely
- an element implying continuance of action.”[1016]
- I suspect that we see a trace of this same expression in what is called
- a derisive or sardonic smile. The lips are then kept joined or almost
- joined, but one corner of the mouth is retracted on the side towards
- the derided person; and this drawing back of the corner is part of a
- true sneer. Although some persons smile more on one side of their face
- than on the other, it is not easy to understand why in cases of
- derision the smile, if a real one, should so commonly be confined to
- one side. I have also on these occasions noticed a slight twitching of
- the muscle which draws up the outer part of the upper lip; and this
- movement, if fully carried out, would have uncovered the canine, and
- would have produced a true sneer.
- Mr. Bulmer, an Australian missionary in a remote part of Gipps’ Land,
- says, in answer to my query about the uncovering of the canine on one
- side, “I find that the natives in snarling at each other speak with the
- teeth closed, the upper lip drawn to one side, and a general angry
- expression of face; but they look direct at the person addressed.”
- Three other observers in Australia, one in Abyssinia, and one in China,
- answer my query on this head in the affirmative; but as the expression
- is rare, and as they enter into no details, I am afraid of implicitly
- trusting them. It is, however, by no means improbable that this
- animal-like expression may be more common with savages than with
- civilized races. Mr. Geach is an observer who may be fully trusted, and
- he has observed it on one occasion in a Malay in the interior of
- Malacca. The Rev. S. O. Glenie answers, “We have observed this
- expression with the natives of Ceylon, but not often.” Lastly, in North
- America, Dr. Rothrock has seen it with some wild Indians, and often in
- a tribe adjoining the Atnahs.
- Although the upper lip is certainly sometimes raised on one side alone
- in sneering at or defying any one, I do not know that this is always
- the case, for the face is commonly half averted, and the expression is
- often momentary. The movement being confined to one side may not be an
- essential part of the expression, but may depend on the proper muscles
- being incapable of movement excepting on one side. I asked four persons
- to endeavour to act voluntarily in this manner; two could expose the
- canine only on the left side, one only on the right side, and the
- fourth on neither side. Nevertheless it is by no means certain that
- these same persons, if defying any one in earnest, would not
- unconsciously have uncovered their canine tooth on the side, whichever
- it might be, towards the offender. For we have seen that some persons
- cannot voluntarily make their eyebrows oblique, yet instantly act in
- this manner when affected by any real, although most trifling, cause of
- distress. The power of voluntarily uncovering the canine on one side of
- the face being thus often wholly lost, indicates that it is a rarely
- used and almost abortive action. It is indeed a surprising fact that
- man should possess the power, or should exhibit any tendency to its
- use; for Mr. Sutton has never noticed a snarling action in our nearest
- allies, namely, the monkeys in the Zoological Gardens, and he is
- positive that the baboons, though furnished with great canines, never
- act thus, but uncover all their teeth when feeling savage and ready for
- an attack. Whether the adult anthropomorphous apes, in the males of
- whom the canines are much larger than in the females, uncover them when
- prepared to fight, is not known.
- The expression here considered, whether that of a playful sneer or
- ferocious snarl, is one of the most curious which occurs in man. It
- reveals his animal descent; for no one, even if rolling on the ground
- in a deadly grapple with an enemy, and attempting to bite him, would
- try to use his canine teeth more than his other teeth. We may readily
- believe from our affinity to the anthropomorphous apes that our male
- semi-human progenitors possessed great canine teeth, and men are now
- occasionally born having them of unusually large size, with interspaces
- in the opposite jaw for their reception.[1017] We may further suspect,
- notwithstanding that we have no support from analogy, that our
- semi-human progenitors uncovered their canine teeth when prepared for
- battle, as we still do when feeling ferocious, or when merely sneering
- at or defying some one, without any intention of making a real attack
- with our teeth.
- CHAPTER XI. DISDAIN—CONTEMPT—DISGUST-GUILT—PRIDE,
- ETC.—HELPLESSNESS—PATIENCE—AFFIRMATION AND NEGATION.
- Contempt, scorn and disdain, variously expressed—Derisive
- smile—Gestures expressive of contempt—Disgust—Guilt, deceit, pride,
- &c.—Helplessness or impotence—Patience—Obstinacy—Shrugging the
- shoulders common to most of the races of man—Signs of affirmation and
- negation.
- Scorn and disdain can hardly be distinguished from contempt, excepting
- that they imply a rather more angry frame of mind. Nor can they be
- clearly distinguished from the feelings discussed in the last chapter
- under the terms of sneering and defiance. Disgust is a sensation rather
- more distinct in its nature and refers to something revolting,
- primarily in relation to the sense of taste, as actually perceived or
- vividly imagined; and secondarily to anything which causes a similar
- feeling, through the sense of smell, touch, and even of eyesight.
- Nevertheless, extreme contempt, or as it is often called loathing
- contempt, hardly differs from disgust. These several conditions of the
- mind are, therefore, nearly related; and each of them may be exhibited
- in many different ways. Some writers have insisted chiefly on one mode
- of expression, and others on a different mode. From this circumstance
- M. Lemoine has argued[1101] that their descriptions are not
- trustworthy. But we shall immediately see that it is natural that the
- feelings which we have here to consider should be expressed in many
- different ways, inasmuch as various habitual actions serve equally
- well, through the principle of association, for their expression.
- Scorn and disdain, as well as sneering and defiance, may be displayed
- by a slight uncovering of the canine tooth on one side of the face; and
- this movement appears to graduate into one closely like a smile. Or the
- smile or laugh may be real, although one of derision; and this implies
- that the offender is so insignificant that he excites only amusement;
- but the amusement is generally a pretence. Gaika in his answers to my
- queries remarks, that contempt is commonly shown by his countrymen, the
- Kafirs, by smiling; and the Rajah Brooke makes the same observation
- with respect to the Dyaks of Borneo. As laughter is primarily the
- expression of simple joy, very young children do not, I believe, ever
- laugh in derision.
- The partial closure of the eyelids, as Duchenne[1102] insists, or the
- turning away of the eyes or of the whole body, are likewise highly
- expressive of disdain. These actions seem to declare that the despised
- person is not worth looking at or is disagreeable to behold. The
- accompanying photograph (Plate V. fig. 1) by Mr. Rejlander, shows this
- form of disdain. It represents a young lady, who is supposed to be
- tearing up the photograph of a despised lover.
- Scorn and Disdain. Plate V
- The most common method of expressing contempt is by movements about the
- nose, or round the mouth; but the latter movements, when strongly
- pronounced, indicate disgust. The nose may be slightly turned up, which
- apparently follows from the turning up of the upper lip; or the
- movement may be abbreviated into the mere wrinkling of the nose. The
- nose is often slightly contracted, so as partly to close the
- passage;[1103] and this is commonly accompanied by a slight snort or
- expiration. All these actions are the same with those which we employ
- when we perceive an offensive odour, and wish to exclude or expel it.
- In extreme cases, as Dr. Piderit remarks,[1104] we protrude and raise
- both lips, or the upper lip alone, so as to close the nostrils as by a
- valve, the nose being thus turned up. We seem thus to say to the
- despised person that he smells offensively,[1105] in nearly the same
- manner as we express to him by half-closing our eyelids, or turning
- away our faces, that he is not worth looking at. It must not, however,
- be supposed that such ideas actually pass through the mind when we
- exhibit our contempt; but as whenever we have perceived a disagreeable
- odour or seen a disagreeable sight, actions of this kind have been
- performed, they have become habitual or fixed, and are now employed
- under any analogous state of mind.
- Various odd little gestures likewise indicate contempt; for instance,
- _snapping one’s fingers_. This, as Mr. Taylor remarks,[1106] “is not
- very intelligible as we generally see it; but when we notice that the
- same sign made quite gently, as if rolling some tiny object away
- between the finger and thumb, or the sign of flipping it away with the
- thumb-nail and forefinger, are usual and well-understood deaf-and-dumb
- gestures, denoting anything tiny, insignificant, contemptible, it seems
- as though we had exaggerated and conventionalized a perfectly natural
- action, so as to lose sight of its original meaning. There is a curious
- mention of this gesture by Strabo.” Mr. Washington Matthews informs me
- that, with the Dakota Indians of North America, contempt is shown not
- only by movements of the face, such as those above described, but
- “conventionally, by the hand being closed and held near the breast,
- then, as the forearm is suddenly extended, the hand is opened and the
- fingers separated from each other. If the person at whose expense the
- sign is made is present, the hand is moved towards him, and the head
- sometimes averted from him.” This sudden extension and opening of the
- hand perhaps indicates the dropping or throwing away a valueless
- object.
- The term ‘disgust,’ in its simplest sense, means something offensive to
- the taste. It is curious how readily this feeling is excited by
- anything unusual in the appearance, odour, or nature of our food. In
- Tierra del Fuego a native touched with his finger some cold preserved
- meat which I was eating at our bivouac, and plainly showed utter
- disgust at its softness; whilst I felt utter disgust at my food being
- touched by a naked savage, though his hands did not appear dirty. A
- smear of soup on a man’s beard looks disgusting, though there is of
- course nothing disgusting in the soup itself. I presume that this
- follows from the strong association in our minds between the sight of
- food, however circumstanced, and the idea of eating it.
- As the sensation of disgust primarily arises in connection with the act
- of eating or tasting, it is natural that its expression should consist
- chiefly in movements round the mouth. But as disgust also causes
- annoyance, it is generally accompanied by a frown, and often by
- gestures as if to push away or to guard oneself against the offensive
- object. In the two photographs (figs. 2 and 3, on Plate V.) Mr.
- Rejlander has simulated this expression with some success. With respect
- to the face, moderate disgust is exhibited in various ways; by the
- mouth being widely opened, as if to let an offensive morsel drop out;
- by spitting; by blowing out of the protruded lips; or by a sound as of
- clearing the throat. Such guttural sounds are written _ach_ or _ugh_;
- and their utterance is sometimes accompanied by a shudder, the arms
- being pressed close to the sides and the shoulders raised in the same
- manner as when horror is experienced.[1107] Extreme disgust is
- expressed by movements round the month identical with those preparatory
- to the act of vomiting. The mouth is opened widely, with the upper lip
- strongly retracted, which wrinkles the sides of the nose, and with the
- lower lip protruded and everted as much as possible. This latter
- movement requires the contraction of the muscles which draw downwards
- the corners of the mouth.[1108]
- It is remarkable how readily and instantly retching or actual vomiting
- is induced in some persons by the mere idea of having partaken of any
- unusual food, as of an animal which is not commonly eaten; although
- there is nothing in such food to cause the stomach to reject it. When
- vomiting results, as a reflex action, from some real cause—as from too
- rich food, or tainted meat, or from an emetic—it does not ensue
- immediately, but generally after a considerable interval of time.
- Therefore, to account for retching or vomiting being so quickly and
- easily excited by a mere idea, the suspicion arises that our
- progenitors must formerly have had the power (like that possessed by
- ruminants and some other animals) of voluntarily rejecting food which
- disagreed with them, or which they thought would disagree with them;
- and now, though this power has been lost, as far as the will is
- concerned, it is called into involuntary action, through the force of a
- formerly well-established habit, whenever the mind revolts at the idea
- of having partaken of any kind of food, or at anything disgusting. This
- suspicion receives support from the fact, of which I am assured by Mr.
- Sutton, that the monkeys in the Zoological Gardens often vomit whilst
- in perfect health, which looks as if the act were voluntary. We can see
- that as man is able to communicate by language to his children and
- others, the knowledge of the kinds of food to be avoided, he would have
- little occasion to use the faculty of voluntary rejection; so that this
- power would tend to be lost through disuse.
- As the sense of smell is so intimately connected with that of taste, it
- is not surprising that an excessively bad odour should excite retching
- or vomiting in some persons, quite as readily as the thought of
- revolting food does; and that, as a further consequence, a moderately
- offensive odour should cause the various expressive movements of
- disgust. The tendency to retch from a fetid odour is immediately
- strengthened in a curious manner by some degree of habit, though soon
- lost by longer familiarity with the cause of offence and by voluntary
- restraint. For instance, I wished to clean the skeleton of a bird,
- which had not been sufficiently macerated, and the smell made my
- servant and myself (we not having had much experience in such work)
- retch so violently, that we were compelled to desist. During the
- previous days I had examined some other skeletons, which smelt
- slightly; yet the odour did not in the least affect me, but,
- subsequently for several days, whenever I handled these same skeletons,
- they made me retch.
- From the answers received from my correspondents it appears that the
- various movements, which have now been described as expressing contempt
- and disgust, prevail throughout a large part of the world. Dr.
- Rothrock, for instance, answers with a decided affirmative with respect
- to certain wild Indian tribes of North America. Crantz says that when a
- Greenlander denies anything with contempt or horror he turns up his
- nose, and gives a slight sound through it.[1109] Mr. Scott has sent me
- a graphic description of the face of a young Hindoo at the sight of
- castor-oil, which he was compelled occasionally to take. Mr. Scott has
- also seen the same expression on the faces of high-caste natives who
- have approached close to some defiling object. Mr. Bridges says that
- the Fuegians “express contempt by shooting out the lips and hissing
- through them, and by turning up the nose.” The tendency either to snort
- through the nose, or to make a noise expressed by _ugh_ or _ach_, is
- noticed by several of my correspondents.
- Spitting seems an almost universal sign of contempt or disgust; and
- spitting obviously represents the rejection of anything offensive from
- the mouth. Shakspeare makes the Duke of Norfolk say, “I spit at
- him—call him a slanderous coward and a villain.” So, again, Falstaff
- says, “Tell thee what, Hal,—if I tell thee a lie, spit in my face.”
- Leichhardt remarks that the Australians “interrupted their speeches by
- spitting, and uttering a noise like pooh! pooh! apparently expressive
- of their disgust.” And Captain Burton speaks of certain negroes
- “spitting with disgust upon the ground.” Captain Speedy informs me that
- this is likewise the case with the Abyssinians. Mr. Geach says that
- with the Malays of Malacca the expression of disgust “answers to
- spitting from the mouth;” and with the Fuegians, according to Mr.
- Bridges “to spit at one is the highest mark of contempt.”[1110]
- I never saw disgust more plainly expressed than on the face of one of
- my infants at the age of five months, when, for the first time, some
- cold water, and again a month afterwards, when a piece of ripe cherry
- was put into his mouth. This was shown by the lips and whole mouth
- assuming a shape which allowed the contents to run or fall quickly out;
- the tongue being likewise protruded. These movements were accompanied
- by a little shudder. It was all the more comical, as I doubt whether
- the child felt real disgust—the eyes and forehead expressing much
- surprise and consideration. The protrusion of the tongue in letting a
- nasty object fall out of the mouth, may explain how it is that lolling
- out the tongue universally serves as a sign of contempt and
- hatred.[1111]
- We have now seen that scorn, disdain, contempt, and disgust are
- expressed in many different ways, by movements of the features, and by
- various gestures; and that these are the same throughout the world.
- They all consist of actions representing the rejection or exclusion of
- some real object which we dislike or abhor, but which does not excite
- in us certain other strong emotions, such as rage or terror; and
- through the force of habit and association similar actions are
- performed, whenever any analogous sensation arises in our minds.
- _Jealousy, Envy, Avarice, Revenge, Suspicion, Deceit, Slyness, Guilt,
- Vanity, Conceit, Ambition, Pride, Humility, &c_.—It is doubtful whether
- the greater number of the above complex states of mind are revealed by
- any fixed expression, sufficiently distinct to be described or
- delineated. When Shakspeare speaks of Envy as _lean-faced_, or _black_,
- or _pale_, and Jealousy as “_the green-eyed monster_;” and when Spenser
- describes Suspicion as “_foul, ill-favoured, and grim_,” they must have
- felt this difficulty. Nevertheless, the above feelings—at least many of
- them—can be detected by the eye; for instance, conceit; but we are
- often guided in a much greater degree than we suppose by our previous
- knowledge of the persons or circumstances.
- My correspondents almost unanimously answer in the affirmative to my
- query, whether the expression of guilt and deceit can be recognized
- amongst the various races of man; and I have confidence in their
- answers, as they generally deny that jealousy can thus be recognized.
- In the cases in which details are given, the eyes are almost always
- referred to. The guilty man is said to avoid looking at his accuser, or
- to give him stolen looks. The eyes are said “to be turned askant,” or
- “to waver from side to side,” or “the eyelids to be lowered and partly
- closed.” This latter remark is made by Mr. Hagenauer with respect to
- the Australians, and by Gaika with respect to the Kafirs. The restless
- movements of the eyes apparently follow, as will be explained when we
- treat of blushing, from the guilty man not enduring to meet the gaze of
- his accuser. I may add, that I have observed a guilty expression,
- without a shade of fear, in some of my own children at a very early
- age. In one instance the expression was unmistakably clear in a child
- two years and seven months old, and led to the detection of his little
- crime. It was shown, as I record in my notes made at the time, by an
- unnatural brightness in the eyes, and by an odd, affected manner,
- impossible to describe.
- Slyness is also, I believe, exhibited chiefly by movements about the
- eyes; for these are less under the control of the will, owing to the
- force of long-continued habit, than are the movements of the body. Mr.
- Herbert Spencer remarks,[1112] “When there is a desire to see something
- on one side of the visual field without being supposed to see it, the
- tendency is to check the conspicuous movement of the head, and to make
- the required adjustment entirely with the eyes; which are, therefore,
- drawn very much to one side. Hence, when the eyes are turned to one
- side, while the face is not turned to the same side, we get the natural
- language of what is called slyness.”
- Of all the above-named complex emotions, Pride, perhaps, is the most
- plainly expressed. A proud man exhibits his sense of superiority over
- others by holding his head and body erect. He is haughty (_haut_), or
- high, and makes himself appear as large as possible; so that
- metaphorically he is said to be swollen or puffed up with pride. A
- peacock or a turkey-cock strutting about with puffed-up feathers, is
- sometimes said to be an emblem of pride.[1113] The arrogant man looks
- down on others, and with lowered eyelids hardly condescends to see
- them; or he may show his contempt by slight movements, such as those
- before described, about the nostrils or lips. Hence the muscle which
- everts the lower lip has been called the _musculus superbus_. In some
- photographs of patients affected by a monomania of pride, sent me by
- Dr. Crichton Browne, the head and body were held erect, and the mouth
- firmly closed. This latter action, expressive of decision, follows, I
- presume, from the proud man feeling perfect self-confidence in himself.
- The whole expression of pride stands in direct antithesis to that of
- humility; so that nothing need here be said of the latter state of
- mind.
- _Helplessness, Impotence: Shrugging the shoulders_.—When a man wishes
- to show that he cannot do something, or prevent something being done,
- he often raises with a quick movement both shoulders. At the same time,
- if the whole gesture is completed, he bends his elbows closely inwards,
- raises his open hands, turning them outwards, with the fingers
- separated. The head is often thrown a little on one side; the eyebrows
- are elevated, and this causes wrinkles across the forehead. The mouth
- is generally opened. I may mention, in order to show how unconsciously
- the features are thus acted on, that though I had often intentionally
- shrugged my shoulders to observe how my arms were placed, I was not at
- all aware that my eyebrows were raised and mouth opened, until I looked
- at myself in a glass; and since then I have noticed the same movements
- in the faces of others. In the accompanying Plate VI., figs. 3 and 4,
- Mr. Rejlander has successfully acted the gesture of shrugging the
- shoulders.
- Englishmen are much less demonstrative than the men of most other
- European nations, and they shrug their shoulders far less frequently
- and energetically than Frenchmen or Italians do. The gesture varies in
- all degrees from the complex movement, just described, to only a
- momentary and scarcely perceptible raising of both shoulders; or, as I
- have noticed in a lady sitting in an arm-chair, to the mere turning
- slightly outwards of the open hands with separated fingers. I have
- never seen very young English children shrug their shoulders, but the
- following case was observed with care by a medical professor and
- excellent observer, and has been communicated to me by him. The father
- of this gentleman was a Parisian, and his mother a Scotch lady. His
- wife is of British extraction on both sides, and my informant does not
- believe that she ever shrugged her shoulders in her life. His children
- have been reared in England, and the nursemaid is a thorough
- Englishwoman, who has never been seen to shrug her shoulders. Now, his
- eldest daughter was observed to shrug her shoulders at the age of
- between sixteen and eighteen months; her mother exclaiming at the time,
- “Look at the little French girl shrugging her shoulders!” At first she
- often acted thus, sometimes throwing her head a little backwards and on
- one side, but she did not, as far as was observed, move her elbows and
- hands in the usual manner. The habit gradually wore away, and now, when
- she is a little over four years old, she is never seen to act thus. The
- father is told that he sometimes shrugs his shoulders, especially when
- arguing with any one; but it is extremely improbable that his daughter
- should have imitated him at so early an age; for, as he remarks, she
- could not possibly have often seen this gesture in him. Moreover, if
- the habit had been acquired through imitation, it is not probable that
- it would so soon have been spontaneously discontinued by this child,
- and, as we shall immediately see, by a second child, though the father
- still lived with his family. This little girl, it may be added,
- resembles her Parisian grandfather in countenance to an almost absurd
- degree. She also presents another and very curious resemblance to him,
- namely, by practising a singular trick. When she impatiently wants
- something, she holds out her little hand, and rapidly rubs the thumb
- against the index and middle finger: now this same trick was frequently
- performed under the same circumstances by her grandfather.
- This gentleman’s second daughter also shrugged her shoulders before the
- age of eighteen months, and afterwards discontinued the habit. It is of
- course possible that she may have imitated her elder sister; but she
- continued it after her sister had lost the habit. She at first
- resembled her Parisian grandfather in a less degree than did her sister
- at the same age, but now in a greater degree. She likewise practises to
- the present time the peculiar habit of rubbing together, when
- impatient, her thumb and two of her fore-fingers.
- In this latter case we have a good instance, like those given in a
- former chapter, of the inheritance of a trick or gesture; for no one, I
- presume, will attribute to mere coincidence so peculiar a habit as
- this, which was common to the grandfather and his two grandchildren who
- had never seen him.
- Considering all the circumstances with reference to these children
- shrugging their shoulders, it can hardly be doubted that they have
- inherited the habit from their French progenitors, although they have
- only one quarter French blood in their veins, and although their
- grandfather did not often shrug his shoulders. There is nothing very
- unusual, though the fact is interesting, in these children having
- gained by inheritance a habit during early youth, and then
- discontinuing it; for it is of frequent occurrence with many kinds of
- animals that certain characters are retained for a period by the young,
- and are then lost.
- As it appeared to me at one time improbable in a high degree that so
- complex a gesture as shrugging the shoulders, together with the
- accompanying movements, should be innate, I was anxious to ascertain
- whether the blind and deaf Laura Bridgman, who could not have learnt
- the habit by imitation, practised it. And I have heard, through Dr.
- Innes, from a lady who has lately had charge of her, that she does
- shrug her shoulders, turn in her elbows, and raise her eyebrows in the
- same manner as other people, and under the same circumstances. I was
- also anxious to learn whether this gesture was practised by the various
- races of man, especially by those who never have had much intercourse
- with Europeans. We shall see that they act in this manner; but it
- appears that the gesture is sometimes confined to merely raising or
- shrugging the shoulders, without the other movements.
- Mr. Scott has frequently seen this gesture in the Bengalees and
- Dhangars (the latter constituting a distinct race) who are employed in
- the Botanic Garden at Calcutta; when, for instance, they have declared
- that they could not do some work, such as lifting a heavy weight. He
- ordered a Bengalee to climb a lofty tree; but the man, with a shrug of
- his shoulders and a lateral shake of his head, said he could not. Mr.
- Scott knowing that the man was lazy, thought he could, and insisted on
- his trying. His face now became pale, his arms dropped to his sides,
- his mouth and eyes were widely opened, and again surveying the tree, he
- looked askant at Mr. Scott, shrugged his shoulders, inverted his
- elbows, extended his open hands, and with a few quick lateral shakes of
- the head declared his inability. Mr. H. Erskine has likewise seen the
- natives of India shrugging their shoulders; but he has never seen the
- elbows turned so much inwards as with us; and whilst shrugging their
- shoulders they sometimes lay their uncrossed hands on their breasts.
- With the wild Malays of the interior of Malacca, and with the Bugis
- (true Malays, though speaking a different language), Mr. Geach has
- often seen this gesture. I presume that it is complete, as, in answer
- to my query descriptive of the movements of the shoulders, arms, hands,
- and face, Mr. Geach remarks, “it is performed in a beautiful style.” I
- have lost an extract from a scientific voyage, in which shrugging the
- shoulders by some natives (Micronesians) of the Caroline Archipelago in
- the Pacific Ocean, was well described. Capt. Speedy informs me that the
- Abyssinians shrug their shoulders but enters into no details. Mrs. Asa
- Gray saw an Arab dragoman in Alexandria acting exactly as described in
- my query, when an old gentleman, on whom he attended, would not go in
- the proper direction which had been pointed out to him.
- Mr. Washington Matthews says, in reference to the wild Indian tribes of
- the western parts of the United States, “I have on a few occasions
- detected men using a slight apologetic shrug, but the rest of the
- demonstration which you describe I have not witnessed.” Fritz Müller
- informs me that he has seen the negroes in Brazil shrugging their
- shoulders; but it is of course possible that they may have learnt to do
- so by imitating the Portuguese. Mrs. Barber has never seen this gesture
- with the Kafirs of South Africa; and Gaika, judging from his answer,
- did not even understand what was meant by my description. Mr. Swinhoe
- is also doubtful about the Chinese; but he has seen them, under the
- circumstances which would make us shrug our shoulders, press their
- right elbow against their side, raise their eyebrows, lift up their
- hand with the palm directed towards the person addressed, and shake it
- from right to left. Lastly, with respect to the Australians, four of my
- informants answer by a simple negative, and one by a simple
- affirmative. Mr. Bunnett, who has had excellent opportunities for
- observation on the borders of the Colony of Victory, also answers by a
- “yes,” adding that the gesture is performed “in a more subdued and less
- demonstrative manner than is the case with civilized nations.” This
- circumstance may account for its not having been noticed by four of my
- informants.
- These statements, relating to Europeans, Hindoos, the hill-tribes of
- India, Malays, Micronesians, Abyssinians, Arabs, Negroes, Indians of
- North America, and apparently to the Australians—many of these natives
- having had scarcely any intercourse with Europeans—are sufficient to
- show that shrugging the shoulders, accompanied in some cases by the
- other proper movements, is a gesture natural to mankind.
- This gesture implies an unintentional or unavoidable action on our own
- part, or one that we cannot perform; or an action performed by another
- person which we cannot prevent. It accompanies such speeches as, “It
- was not my fault;” “It is impossible for me to grant this favour;” “He
- must follow his own course, I cannot stop him.” Shrugging the shoulders
- likewise expresses patience, or the absence of any intention to resist.
- Hence the muscles which raise the shoulders are sometimes called, as I
- have been informed by an artist, the patience muscles. Shylock the Jew,
- says,
- “Signor Antonio, many a time and oft
- In the Rialto have you rated me
- About my monies and usances;
- Still have I borne it with a patient shrug.”
- _Merchant of Venice_, act i. sc. 3.
- Sir C. Bell has given[1114] a life-like figure of a man, who is
- shrinking back from some terrible danger, and is on the point of
- screaming out in abject terror. He is represented with his shoulders
- lifted up almost to his ears; and this at once declares that there is
- no thought of resistance.
- As shrugging the shoulders generally implies “I cannot do this or
- that,” so by a slight change, it sometimes implies “I won’t do it.” The
- movement then expresses a dogged determination not to act. Olmsted
- describes[1115] an Indian in Texas as giving a great shrug to his
- shoulders, when he was informed that a party of men were Germans and
- not Americans, thus expressing that he would have nothing to do with
- them. Sulky and obstinate children may be seen with both their
- shoulders raised high up; but this movement is not associated with the
- others which generally accompany a true shrug. An excellent
- observer[1116] in describing a young man who was determined not to
- yield to his father’s desire, says, “He thrust his hands deep down into
- his pockets, and set up his shoulders to his ears, which was a good
- warning that, come right or wrong, this rock should fly from its firm
- base as soon as Jack would; and that any remonstrance on the subject
- was purely futile.” As soon as the son got his own way, he “put his
- shoulders into their natural position.”
- Resignation is sometimes shown by the open hands being placed, one over
- the other, on the lower part of the body. I should not have thought
- this little gesture worth even a passing notice, had not Dr. W. Ogle
- remarked to me that he had two or three times observed it in patients
- who were preparing for operations under chloroform. They exhibited no
- great fear, but seemed to declare by this posture of their hands, that
- they had made up their minds, and were resigned to the inevitable.
- We may now inquire why men in all parts of the world when they
- feel,—whether or not they wish to show this feeling,—that they cannot
- or will not do something, or will not resist something if done by
- another, shrug their shoulders, at the same time often bending in their
- elbows, showing the palms of their hands with extended fingers, often
- throwing their heads a little on one side, raising their eyebrows, and
- opening their mouths. These states of the mind are either simply
- passive, or show a determination not to act. None of the above
- movements are of the least service. The explanation lies, I cannot
- doubt, in the principle of unconscious antithesis. This principle here
- seems to come into play as clearly as in the case of a dog, who, when
- feeling savage, puts himself in the proper attitude for attacking and
- for making himself appear terrible to his enemy; but as soon as he
- feels affectionate, throws his whole body into a directly opposite
- attitude, though this is of no direct use to him.
- Let it be observed how an indignant man, who resents, and will not
- submit to some injury, holds his head erect, squares his shoulders, and
- expands his chest. He often clenches his fists, and puts one or both
- arms in the proper position for attack or defence, with the muscles of
- his limbs rigid. He frowns,—that is, he contracts and lowers his
- brows,—and, being determined, closes his mouth. The actions and
- attitude of a helpless man are, in every one of these respects, exactly
- the reverse. In Plate VI. we may imagine one of the figures on the left
- side to have just said, “What do you mean by insulting me?” and one of
- the figures on the right side to answer, “I really could not help it.”
- The helpless man unconsciously contracts the muscles of his forehead
- which are antagonistic to those that cause a frown, and thus raises his
- eyebrows; at the same time he relaxes the muscles about the mouth, so
- that the lower jaw drops. The antithesis is complete in every detail,
- not only in the movements of the features, but in the position of the
- limbs and in the attitude of the whole body, as may be seen in the
- accompanying plate. As the helpless or apologetic man often wishes to
- show his state of mind, he then acts in a conspicuous or demonstrative
- manner.
- In accordance with the fact that squaring the elbows and clenching the
- fists are gestures by no means universal with the men of all races,
- when they feel indignant and are prepared to attack their enemy, so it
- appears that a helpless or apologetic frame of mind is expressed in
- many parts of the world by merely shrugging the shoulders, without
- turning inwards the elbows and opening the hands. The man or child who
- is obstinate, or one who is resigned to some great misfortune, has in
- neither case any idea of resistance by active means; and he expresses
- this state of mind, by simply keeping his shoulders raised; or he may
- possibly fold his arms across his breast.
- _Signs of affirmation or approval, and of negation or disapproval:
- nodding and shaking the head_.—I was curious to ascertain how far the
- common signs used by us in affirmation and negation were general
- throughout the world. These signs are indeed to a certain extent
- expressive of our feelings, as we give a vertical nod of approval with
- a smile to our children, when we approve of their conduct; and shake
- our heads laterally with a frown, when we disapprove. With infants, the
- first act of denial consists in refusing food; and I repeatedly noticed
- with my own infants, that they did so by withdrawing their heads
- laterally from the breast, or from anything offered them in a spoon. In
- accepting food and taking it into their mouths, they incline their
- heads forwards. Since making these observations I have been informed
- that the same idea had occurred to Charma.[1117] It deserves notice
- that in accepting or taking food, there is only a single movement
- forward, and a single nod implies an affirmation. On the other hand, in
- refusing food, especially if it be pressed on them, children frequently
- move their heads several times from side to side, as we do in shaking
- our heads in negation. Moreover, in the case of refusal, the head is
- not rarely thrown backwards, or the mouth is closed, so that these
- movements might likewise come to serve as signs of negation. Mr.
- Wedgwood remarks on this subject,[1118] that “when the voice is exerted
- with closed teeth or lips, it produces the sound of the letter _n_ or
- _m_. Hence we may account for the use of the particle _ne_ to signify
- negation, and possibly also of the Greek mh in the same sense.”
- That these signs are innate or instinctive, at least with Anglo-Saxons,
- is rendered highly probable by the blind and deaf Laura Bridgman
- “constantly accompanying her _yes_ with the common affirmative nod, and
- her _no_ with our negative shake of the head.” Had not Mr. Lieber
- stated to the contrary,[1119] I should have imagined that these
- gestures might have been acquired or learnt by her, considering her
- wonderful sense of touch and appreciation of the movements of others.
- With microcephalous idiots, who are so degraded that they never learn
- to speak, one of them is described by Vogt,[1120] as answering, when
- asked whether he wished for more food or drink, by inclining or shaking
- his head. Schmalz, in his remarkable dissertation on the education of
- the deaf and dumb, as well as of children raised only one degree above
- idiotcy, assumes that they can always both make and understand the
- common signs of affirmation and negation.[1121]
- Nevertheless if we look to the various races of man, these signs are
- not so universally employed as I should have expected; yet they seem
- too general to be ranked as altogether conventional or artificial. My
- informants assert that both signs are used by the Malays, by the
- natives of Ceylon, the Chinese, the negroes of the Guinea coast, and,
- according to Gaika, by the Kafirs of South Africa, though with these
- latter people Mrs. Barber has never seen a lateral shake used as a
- negative. With respect to the Australians, seven observers agree that a
- nod is given in affirmation; five agree about a lateral shake in
- negation, accompanied or not by some word; but Mr. Dyson Lacy has never
- seen this latter sign in Queensland, and Mr. Bulmer says that in Gipps’
- Land a negative is expressed by throwing the head a little backwards
- and putting out the tongue. At the northern extremity of the continent,
- near Torres Straits, the natives when uttering a negative “don’t shake
- the head with it, but holding up the right hand, shake it by turning it
- half round and back again two or three times.”[1122] The throwing back
- of the head with a cluck of the tongue is said to be used as a negative
- by the modern Greeks and Turks, the latter people expressing _yes_ by a
- movement like that made by us when we shake our heads.[1123] The
- Abyssinians, as I am informed by Captain Speedy, express a negative by
- jerking the head to the right shoulder, together with a slight cluck,
- the mouth being closed; an affirmation is expressed by the head being
- thrown backwards and the eyebrows raised for an instant. The Tagals of
- Luzon, in the Philippine Archipelago, as I hear from Dr. Adolf Meyer,
- when they say “yes,” also throw the head backwards. According to the
- Rajah Brooke, the Dyaks of Borneo express an affirmation by raising the
- eyebrows, and a negation by slightly contracting them, together with a
- peculiar look from the eyes. With the Arabs on the Nile, Professor and
- Mrs. Asa Gray concluded that nodding in affirmation was rare, whilst
- shaking the head in negation was never used, and was not even
- understood by them. With the Esquimaux[1124] a nod means _yes_ and a
- wink _no_. The New Zealanders “elevate the head and chin in place of
- nodding acquiescence.”[1125]
- With the Hindoos Mr. H. Erskine concludes from inquiries made from
- experienced Europeans, and from native gentlemen, that the signs of
- affirmation and negation vary—a nod and a lateral shake being sometimes
- used as we do; but a negative is more commonly expressed by the head
- being thrown suddenly backwards and a little to one side, with a cluck
- of the tongue. What the meaning may be of this cluck of the tongue,
- which has been observed with various people, I cannot imagine. A native
- gentleman stated that affirmation is frequently shown by the head being
- thrown to the left. I asked Mr. Scott to attend particularly to this
- point, and, after repeated observations, he believes that a vertical
- nod is not commonly used by the natives in affirmation, but that the
- head is first thrown backwards either to the left or right, and then
- jerked obliquely forwards only once. This movement would perhaps have
- been described by a less careful observer as a lateral shake. He also
- states that in negation the head is usually held nearly upright, and
- shaken several times.
- Mr. Bridges informs me that the Fuegians nod their heads vertically in
- affirmation, and shake them laterally in denial. With the wild Indians
- of North America, according to Mr. Washington Matthews, nodding and
- shaking the head have been learnt from Europeans, and are not naturally
- employed. They express affirmation by describing with the hand (all the
- fingers except the index being flexed) a curve downwards and outwards
- from the body, whilst negation is expressed by moving the open hand
- outwards, with the palm facing inwards. Other observers state that the
- sign of affirmation with these Indians is the forefinger being raised,
- and then lowered and pointed to the ground, or the hand is waved
- straight forward from the face; and that the sign of negation is the
- finger or whole hand shaken from side to side.[1126] This latter
- movement probably represents in all cases the lateral shaking of the
- head. The Italians are said in like manner to move the lifted finger
- from right to left in negation, as indeed we English sometimes do.
- On the whole we find considerable diversity in the signs of affirmation
- and negation in the different races of man. With respect to negation,
- if we admit that the shaking of the finger or hand from side to side is
- symbolic of the lateral movement of the head; and if we admit that the
- sudden backward movement of the head represents one of the actions
- often practised by young children in refusing food, then there is much
- uniformity throughout the world in the signs of negation, and we can
- see how they originated. The most marked exceptions are presented by
- the Arabs, Esquimaux, some Australian tribes, and Dyaks. With the
- latter a frown is the sign of negation, and with us frowning often
- accompanies a lateral shake of the head.
- With respect to nodding in affirmation, the exceptions are rather more
- numerous, namely with some of the Hindoos, with the Turks, Abyssinians,
- Dyaks, Tagals, and New Zealanders. The eyebrows are sometimes raised in
- affirmation, and as a person in bending his head forwards and downwards
- naturally looks up to the person whom he addresses, he will be apt to
- raise his eyebrows, and this sign may thus have arisen as an
- abbreviation. So again with the New Zealanders, the lifting up the chin
- and head in affirmation may perhaps represent in an abbreviated form
- the upward movement of the head after it has been nodded forwards and
- downwards.
- CHAPTER XII. SURPRISE—ASTONISHMENT—FEAR—HORROR.
- Surprise, astonishment—Elevation of the eyebrows—Opening the
- mouth—Protrusion of the lips—Gestures accompanying
- surprise—Admiration—Fear—Terror—Erection of the hair—Contraction of the
- platysma muscle—Dilatation of the pupils—Horror—Conclusion.
- Attention, if sudden and close, graduates into surprise; and this into
- astonishment; and this into stupefied amazement. The latter frame of
- mind is closely akin to terror. Attention is shown by the eyebrows
- being slightly raised; and as this state increases into surprise, they
- are raised to a much greater extent, with the eyes and mouth widely
- open. The raising of the eyebrows is necessary in order that the eyes
- should be opened quickly and widely; and this movement produces
- transverse wrinkles across the forehead. The degree to which the eyes
- and mouth are opened corresponds with the degree of surprise felt; but
- these movements must be coordinated; for a widely opened mouth with
- eyebrows only slightly raised results in a meaningless grimace, as Dr.
- Duchenne has shown in one of his photographs.[1201] On the other hand,
- a person may often be seen to pretend surprise by merely raising his
- eyebrows.
- Dr. Duchenne has given a photograph of an old man with his eyebrows
- well elevated and arched by the galvanization of the frontal muscle;
- and with his mouth voluntarily opened. This figure expresses surprise
- with much truth. I showed it to twenty-four persons without a word of
- explanation, and one alone did not at all understand what was intended.
- A second person answered terror, which is not far wrong; some of the
- others, however, added to the words surprise or astonishment, the
- epithets horrified, woful, painful, or disgusted.
- The eyes and mouth being widely open is an expression universally
- recognized as one of surprise or astonishment. Thus Shakespeare says,
- “I saw a smith stand with open mouth swallowing a tailor’s news.”
- (‘King John,’ act iv. scene ii.) And again, “They seemed almost, with
- staring on one another, to tear the cases of their eyes; there was
- speech in the dumbness, language in their very gesture; they looked as
- they had heard of a world destroyed.” (‘Winter’s Tale,’ act v. scene
- ii.)
- My informants answer with remarkable uniformity to the same effect,
- with respect to the various races of man; the above movements of the
- features being often accompanied by certain gestures and sounds,
- presently to be described. Twelve observers in different parts of
- Australia agree on this head. Mr. Winwood Reade has observed this
- expression with the negroes on the Guinea coast. The chief Gaika and
- others answer _yes_ to my query with respect to the Kafirs of South
- Africa; and so do others emphatically with reference to the
- Abyssinians, Ceylonese, Chinese, Fuegians, various tribes of North
- America, and New Zealanders. With the latter, Mr. Stack states that the
- expression is more plainly shown by certain individuals than by others,
- though all endeavour as much as possible to conceal their feelings. The
- Dyaks of Borneo are said by the Rajah Brooke to open their eyes widely,
- when astonished, often swinging their heads to and fro, and beating
- their breasts. Mr. Scott informs me that the workmen in the Botanic
- Gardens at Calcutta are strictly ordered not to smoke; but they often
- disobey this order, and when suddenly surprised in the act, they first
- open their eyes and mouths widely. They then often slightly shrug their
- shoulders, as they perceive that discovery is inevitable, or frown and
- stamp on the ground from vexation. Soon they recover from their
- surprise, and abject fear is exhibited by the relaxation of all their
- muscles; their heads seem to sink between their shoulders; their fallen
- eyes wander to and fro; and they supplicate forgiveness.
- The well-known Australian explorer, Mr. Stuart, has given[1202] a
- striking account of stupefied amazement together with terror in a
- native who had never before seen a man on horseback. Mr. Stuart
- approached unseen and called to him from a little distance. “He turned
- round and saw me. What he imagined I was I do not know; but a finer
- picture of fear and astonishment I never saw. He stood incapable of
- moving a limb, riveted to the spot, mouth open and eyes staring.... He
- remained motionless until our black got within a few yards of him, when
- suddenly throwing down his waddies, he jumped into a mulga bush as high
- as he could get.” He could not speak, and answered not a word to the
- inquiries made by the black, but, trembling from head to foot, “waved
- with his hand for us to be off.”
- That the eyebrows are raised by an innate or instinctive impulse may be
- inferred from the fact that Laura Bridgman invariably acts thus when
- astonished, as I have been assured by the lady who has lately had
- charge of her. As surprise is excited by something unexpected or
- unknown, we naturally desire, when startled, to perceive the cause as
- quickly as possible; and we consequently open our eyes fully, so that
- the field of vision may be increased, and the eyeballs moved easily in
- any direction. But this hardly accounts for the eyebrows being so
- greatly raised as is the case, and for the wild staring of the open
- eyes. The explanation lies, I believe, in the impossibility of opening
- the eyes with great rapidity by merely raising the upper lids. To
- effect this the eyebrows must be lifted energetically. Any one who will
- try to open his eyes as quickly as possible before a mirror will find
- that he acts thus; and the energetic lifting up of the eyebrows opens
- the eyes so widely that they stare, the white being exposed all round
- the iris. Moreover, the elevation of the eyebrows is an advantage in
- looking upwards; for as long as they are lowered they impede our vision
- in this direction. Sir C. Bell gives[1203] a curious little proof of
- the part which the eyebrows play in opening the eyelids. In a stupidly
- drunken man all the muscles are relaxed, and the eyelids consequently
- droop, in the same manner as when we are falling asleep. To counteract
- this tendency the drunkard raises his eyebrows; and this gives to him a
- puzzled, foolish look, as is well represented in one of Hogarth’s
- drawings. The habit of raising the eyebrows having once been gained in
- order to see as quickly as possible all around us, the movement would
- follow from the force of association whenever astonishment was felt
- from any cause, even from a sudden sound or an idea.
- With adult persons, when the eyebrows are raised, the whole forehead
- becomes much wrinkled in transverse lines; but with children this
- occurs only to a slight degree. The wrinkles run in lines concentric
- with each eyebrow, and are partially confluent in the middle. They are
- highly characteristic of the expression of surprise or astonishment.
- Each eyebrow, when raised, becomes also, as Duchenne remarks,[1204]
- more arched than it was before.
- The cause of the mouth being opened when astonishment is felt, is a
- much more complex affair; and several causes apparently concur in
- leading to this movement. It has often been supposed[1205] that the
- sense of hearing is thus rendered more acute; but I have watched
- persons listening intently to a slight noise, the nature and source of
- which they knew perfectly, and they did not open their mouths.
- Therefore I at one time imagined that the open mouth might aid in
- distinguishing the direction whence a sound proceeded, by giving
- another channel for its entrance into the ear through the eustachian
- tube, But Dr. W. Ogle[1206] has been so kind as to search the best
- recent authorities on the functions of the eustachian tube, and he
- informs me that it is almost conclusively proved that it remains closed
- except during the act of deglutition; and that in persons in whom the
- tube remains abnormally open, the sense of hearing, as far as external
- sounds are concerned, is by no means improved; on the contrary, it is
- impaired by the respiratory sounds being rendered more distinct. If a
- watch be placed within the mouth, but not allowed to touch the sides,
- the ticking is heard much less plainly than when held outside. In
- persons in whom from disease or a cold the eustachian tube is
- permanently or temporarily closed, the sense of hearing is injured; but
- this may be accounted for by mucus accumulating within the tube, and
- the consequent exclusion of air. We may therefore infer that the mouth
- is not kept open under the sense of astonishment for the sake of
- hearing sounds more distinctly; notwithstanding that most deaf people
- keep their mouths open.
- Every sudden emotion, including astonishment, quickens the action of
- the heart, and with it the respiration. Now we can breathe, as
- Gratiolet remarks[1207] and as appears to me to be the case, much more
- quietly through the open mouth than through the nostrils. Therefore,
- when we wish to listen intently to any sound, we either stop breathing,
- or breathe as quietly as possible, by opening our mouths, at the same
- time keeping our bodies motionless. One of my sons was awakened in the
- night by a noise under circumstances which naturally led to great care,
- and after a few minutes he perceived that his mouth was widely open. He
- then became conscious that he had opened it for the sake of breathing
- as quietly as possible. This view receives support from the reversed
- case which occurs with dogs. A dog when panting after exercise, or on a
- hot day, breathes loudly; but if his attention be suddenly aroused, he
- instantly pricks his ears to listen, shuts his mouth, and breathes
- quietly, as he is enabled to do, through his nostrils.
- When the attention is concentrated for a length of time with fixed
- earnestness on any object or subject, all the organs of the body are
- forgotten and neglected;[1208] and as the nervous energy of each
- individual is limited in amount, little is transmitted to any part of
- the system, excepting that which is at the time brought into energetic
- action. Therefore many of the muscles tend to become relaxed, and the
- jaw drops from its own weight. This will account for the dropping of
- the jaw and open mouth of a man stupefied with amazement, and perhaps
- when less strongly affected. I have noticed this appearance, as I find
- recorded in my notes, in very young children when they were only
- moderately surprised.
- There is still another and highly effective cause, leading to the mouth
- being opened, when we are astonished, and more especially when we are
- suddenly startled. We can draw a full and deep inspiration much more
- easily through the widely open mouth than through the nostrils. Now
- when we start at any sudden sound or sight, almost all the muscles of
- the body are involuntarily and momentarily thrown into strong action,
- for the sake of guarding ourselves against or jumping away from the
- danger, which we habitually associate with anything unexpected. But we
- always unconsciously prepare ourselves for any great exertion, as
- formerly explained, by first taking a deep and full inspiration, and we
- consequently open our mouths. If no exertion follows, and we still
- remain astonished, we cease for a time to breathe, or breathe as
- quietly as possible, in order that every sound may be distinctly heard.
- Or again, if our attention continues long and earnestly absorbed, all
- our muscles become relaxed, and the jaw, which was at first suddenly
- opened, remains dropped. Thus several causes concur towards this same
- movement, whenever surprise, astonishment, or amazement is felt.
- Although when thus affected, our mouths are generally opened, yet the
- lips are often a little protruded. This fact reminds us of the same
- movement, though in a much more strongly marked degree, in the
- chimpanzee and orang when astonished. As a strong expiration naturally
- follows the deep inspiration which accompanies the first sense of
- startled surprise, and as the lips are often protruded, the various
- sounds which are then commonly uttered can apparently be accounted for.
- But sometimes a strong expiration alone is heard; thus Laura Bridgman,
- when amazed, rounds and protrudes her lips, opens them, and breathes
- strongly.[1209] One of the commonest sounds is a deep _Oh_; and this
- would naturally follow, as explained by Helmholtz, from the mouth being
- moderately opened and the lips protruded. On a quiet night some rockets
- were fired from the ‘Beagle,’ in a little creek at Tahiti, to amuse the
- natives; and as each rocket, was let off there was absolute silence,
- but this was invariably followed by a deep groaning _Oh_, resounding
- all round the bay. Mr. Washington Matthews says that the North American
- Indians express astonishment by a groan; and the negroes on the West
- Coast of Africa, according to Mr. Winwood Reade, protrude their lips,
- and make a sound like _heigh, heigh_. If the mouth is not much opened,
- whilst the lips are considerably protruded, a blowing, hissing, or
- whistling noise is produced. Mr. R. Brough Smith informs me that an
- Australian from the interior was taken to the theatre to see an acrobat
- rapidly turning head over heels: “he was greatly astonished, and
- protruded his lips, making a noise with his mouth as if blowing out a
- match.” According to Mr. Bulmer the Australians, when surprised, utter
- the exclamation _korki_, “and to do this the mouth is drawn out as if
- going to whistle.” We Europeans often whistle as a sign of surprise;
- thus, in a recent novel[1210] it is said, “here the man expressed his
- astonishment and disapprobation by a prolonged whistle.” A Kafir girl,
- as Mr. J. Mansel Weale informs me, “on hearing of the high price of an
- article, raised her eyebrows and whistled just as a European would.”
- Mr. Wedgwood remarks that such sounds are written down as _whew_, and
- they serve as interjections for surprise.
- According to three other observers, the Australians often evince
- astonishment by a clucking noise. Europeans also sometimes express
- gentle surprise by a little clicking noise of nearly the same kind. We
- have seen that when we are startled, the mouth is suddenly opened; and
- if the tongue happens to be then pressed closely against the palate,
- its sudden withdrawal will produce a sound of this kind, which might
- thus come to express surprise.
- Gestures of the Body. Plate VII
- Turning to gestures of the body. A surprised person often raises his
- opened hands high above his head, or by bending his arms only to the
- level of his face. The flat palms are directed towards the person who
- causes this feeling, and the straightened fingers are separated. This
- gesture is represented by Mr. Rejlander in Plate VII. fig. 1. In the
- ‘Last Supper,’ by Leonardo da Vinci, two of the Apostles have their
- hands half uplifted, clearly expressive of their astonishment. A
- trustworthy observer told me that he had lately met his wife under most
- unexpected circumstances: “She started, opened her mouth and eyes very
- widely, and threw up both her arms above her head.” Several years ago I
- was surprised by seeing several of my young children earnestly doing
- something together on the ground; but the distance was too great for me
- to ask what they were about. Therefore I threw up my open hands with
- extended fingers above my head; and as soon as I had done this, I
- became conscious of the action. I then waited, without saying a word,
- to see if my children had understood this gesture; and as they came
- running to me they cried out, “We saw that you were astonished at us.”
- I do not know whether this gesture is common to the various races of
- man, as I neglected to make inquiries on this head. That it is innate
- or natural may be inferred from the fact that Laura Bridgman, when
- amazed, “spreads her arms and turns her hands with extended fingers
- upwards;”[1211] nor is it likely, considering that the feeling of
- surprise is generally a brief one, that she should have learnt this
- gesture through her keen sense of touch.
- Huschke describes[1212] a somewhat different yet allied gesture, which
- he says is exhibited by persons when astonished. They hold themselves
- erect, with the features as before described, but with the straightened
- arms extended backwards—the stretched fingers being separated from each
- other. I have never myself seen this gesture; but Huschke is probably
- correct; for a friend asked another man how he would express great
- astonishment, and he at once threw himself into this attitude.
- These gestures are, I believe, explicable on the principle of
- antithesis. We have seen that an indignant man holds his head erect,
- squares his shoulders, turns out his elbows, often clenches his fist,
- frowns, and closes his mouth; whilst the attitude of a helpless man is
- in every one of these details the reverse. Now, a man in an ordinary
- frame of mind, doing nothing and thinking of nothing in particular,
- usually keeps his two arms suspended laxly by his sides, with his hands
- somewhat flexed, and the fingers near together. Therefore, to raise the
- arms suddenly, either the whole arms or the fore-arms, to open the
- palms flat, and to separate the fingers,—or, again, to straighten the
- arms, extending them backwards with separated fingers,—are movements in
- complete antithesis to those preserved under an indifferent frame of
- mind, and they are, in consequence, unconsciously assumed by an
- astonished man. There is, also, often a desire to display surprise in a
- conspicuous manner, and the above attitudes are well fitted for this
- purpose. It may be asked why should surprise, and only a few other
- states of the mind, be exhibited by movements in antithesis to others.
- But this principle will not be brought into play in the case of those
- emotions, such as terror, great joy, suffering, or rage, which
- naturally lead to certain lines of action and produce certain effects
- on the body, for the whole system is thus preoccupied; and these
- emotions are already thus expressed with the greatest plainness.
- There is another little gesture, expressive of astonishment of which I
- can offer no explanation; namely, the hand being placed over the mouth
- or on some part of the head. This has been observed with so many races
- of man, that it must have some natural origin. A wild Australian was
- taken into a large room full of official papers, which surprised him
- greatly, and he cried out, _cluck, cluck, cluck_, putting the back of
- his hand towards his lips. Mrs. Barber says that the Kafirs and Fingoes
- express astonishment by a serious look and by placing the right hand
- upon the mouth, uttering the word _mawo_, which means ‘wonderful.’ The
- Bushmen are said[1213] to put their right hands to their necks, bending
- their heads backwards. Mr. Winwood Reade has observed that the negroes
- on the West Coast of Africa, when surprised, clap their hands to their
- mouths, saying at the same time, “My mouth cleaves to me,” i. e. to my
- hands; and he has heard that this is their usual gesture on such
- occasions. Captain Speedy informs me that the Abyssinians place their
- right hand to the forehead, with the palm outside. Lastly, Mr.
- Washington Matthews states that the conventional sign of astonishment
- with the wild tribes of the western parts of the United States “is made
- by placing the half-closed hand over the mouth; in doing this, the head
- is often bent forwards, and words or low groans are sometimes uttered.”
- Catlin[1214] makes the same remark about the hand being pressed over
- the mouth by the Mandans and other Indian tribes.
- _Admiration_.—Little need be said on this head. Admiration apparently
- consists of surprise associated with some pleasure and a sense of
- approval. When vividly felt, the eyes are opened and the eyebrows
- raised; the eyes become bright, instead of remaining blank, as under
- simple astonishment; and the mouth, instead of gaping open, expands
- into a smile.
- _Fear, Terror_.—The word ‘fear’ seems to be derived from what is sudden
- and dangerous;[1215] and that of terror from the trembling of the vocal
- organs and body. I use the word ‘terror’ for extreme fear; but some
- writers think it ought to be confined to cases in which the imagination
- is more particularly concerned. Fear is often preceded by astonishment,
- and is so far akin to it, that both lead to the senses of sight and
- hearing being instantly aroused. In both cases the eyes and mouth are
- widely opened, and the eyebrows raised. The frightened man at first
- stands like a statue motionless and breathless, or crouches down as if
- instinctively to escape observation.
- The heart beats quickly and violently, so that it palpitates or knocks
- against the ribs; but it is very doubtful whether it then works more
- efficiently than usual, so as to send a greater supply of blood to all
- parts of the body; for the skin instantly becomes pale, as during
- incipient faintness. This paleness of the surface, however, is probably
- in large part, or exclusively, due to the vasomotor centre being
- affected in such a manner as to cause the contraction of the small
- arteries of the skin. That the skin is much affected under the sense of
- great fear, we see in the marvellous and inexplicable manner in which
- perspiration immediately exudes from it. This exudation is all the more
- remarkable, as the surface is then cold, and hence the term a cold
- sweat; whereas, the sudorific glands are properly excited into action
- when the surface is heated. The hairs also on the skin stand erect; and
- the superficial muscles shiver. In connection with the disturbed action
- of the heart, the breathing is hurried. The salivary glands act
- imperfectly; the mouth becomes dry,[1216] and is often opened and shut.
- I have also noticed that under slight fear there is a strong tendency
- to yawn. One of the best-marked symptoms is the trembling of all the
- muscles of the body; and this is often first seen in the lips. From
- this cause, and from the dryness of the mouth, the voice becomes husky
- or indistinct, or may altogether fail. “Obstupui, steteruntque comae,
- et vox faucibus haesit.”
- Of vague fear there is a well-known and grand description in Job:—“In
- thoughts from the visions of the night, when deep sleep falleth on men,
- fear came upon me, and trembling, which made all my bones to shake.
- Then a spirit passed before my face; the hair of my flesh stood up. It
- stood still, but I could not discern the form thereof: an image was
- before my eyes, there was silence, and I heard a voice, saying, Shall
- mortal man be more just than God? Shall a man be more pure than his
- Maker?” (Job iv. 13)
- As fear increases into an agony of terror, we behold, as under all
- violent emotions, diversified results. The heart beats wildly, or may
- fail to act and faintness ensue; there is a death-like pallor; the
- breathing is laboured; the wings of the nostrils are wildly dilated;
- “there is a gasping and convulsive motion of the lips, a tremor on the
- hollow cheek, a gulping and catching of the throat;”[1217] the
- uncovered and protruding eyeballs are fixed on the object of terror; or
- they may roll restlessly from side to side, _huc illuc volvens oculos
- totumque pererrat_.[1218] The pupils are said to be enormously dilated.
- All the muscles of the body may become rigid, or may be thrown into
- convulsive movements. The hands are alternately clenched and opened,
- often with a twitching movement. The arms may be protruded, as if to
- avert some dreadful danger, or may be thrown wildly over the head. The
- Rev. Mr. Hagenauer has seen this latter action in a terrified
- Australian. In other cases there is a sudden and uncontrollable
- tendency to headlong flight; and so strong is this, that the boldest
- soldiers may be seized with a sudden panic.
- As fear rises to an extreme pitch, the dreadful scream of terror is
- heard. Great beads of sweat stand on the skin. All the muscles of the
- body are relaxed. Utter prostration soon follows, and the mental powers
- fail. The intestines are affected. The sphincter muscles cease to act,
- and no longer retain the contents of the body.
- Photograph of an Insane Woman. Fig. 19
- Dr. J. Crichton Browne has given me so striking an account of intense
- fear in an insane woman, aged thirty-five, that the description though
- painful ought not to be omitted. When a paroxysm seizes her, she
- screams out, “This is hell!” “There is a black woman!” “I can’t get
- out!”—and other such exclamations. When thus screaming, her movements
- are those of alternate tension and tremor. For one instant she clenches
- her hands, holds her arms out before her in a stiff semi-flexed
- position; then suddenly bends her body forwards, sways rapidly to and
- fro, draws her fingers through her hair, clutches at her neck, and
- tries to tear off her clothes. The sterno-cleido-mastoid muscles (which
- serve to bend the head on the chest) stand out prominently, as if
- swollen, and the skin in front of them is much wrinkled. Her hair,
- which is cut short at the back of her head, and is smooth when she is
- calm, now stands on end; that in front being dishevelled by the
- movements of her hands. The countenance expresses great mental agony.
- The skin is flushed over the face and neck, down to the clavicles, and
- the veins of the forehead and neck stand out like thick cords. The
- lower lip drops, and is somewhat everted. The mouth is kept half open,
- with the lower jaw projecting. The cheeks are hollow and deeply
- furrowed in curved lines running from the wings of the nostrils to the
- corners of the mouth. The nostrils themselves are raised and extended.
- The eyes are widely opened, and beneath them the skin appears swollen;
- the pupils are large. The forehead is wrinkled transversely in many
- folds, and at the inner extremities of the eyebrows it is strongly
- furrowed in diverging lines, produced by the powerful and persistent
- contraction of the corrugators.
- Terror. Fig. 20
- Mr. Bell has also described[1219] an agony of terror and of despair,
- which he witnessed in a murderer, whilst carried to the place of
- execution in Turin. “On each side of the car the officiating priests
- were seated; and in the centre sat the criminal himself. It was
- impossible to witness the condition of this unhappy wretch without
- terror; and yet, as if impelled by some strange infatuation, it was
- equally impossible not to gaze upon an object so wild, so full of
- horror. He seemed about thirty-five years of age; of large and muscular
- form; his countenance marked by strong and savage features; half naked,
- pale as death, agonized with terror, every limb strained in anguish,
- his hands clenched convulsively, the sweat breaking out on his bent and
- contracted brow, he kissed incessantly the figure of our Saviour,
- painted on the flag which was suspended before him; but with an agony
- of wildness and despair, of which nothing ever exhibited on the stage
- can give the slightest conception.”
- I will add only one other case, illustrative of a man utterly
- prostrated by terror. An atrocious murderer of two persons was brought
- into a hospital, under the mistaken impression that he had poisoned
- himself; and Dr. W. Ogle carefully watched him the next morning, while
- he was being handcuffed and taken away by the police. His pallor was
- extreme, and his prostration so great that he was hardly able to dress
- himself. His skin perspired; and his eyelids and head drooped so much
- that it was impossible to catch even a glimpse of his eyes. His lower
- jaw hung down. There was no contraction of any facial muscle, and Dr.
- Ogle is almost certain that the hair did not stand on end, for he
- observed it narrowly, as it had been dyed for the sake of concealment.
- With respect to fear, as exhibited by the various races of man, my
- informants agree that the signs are the same as with Europeans. They
- are displayed in an exaggerated degree with the Hindoos and natives of
- Ceylon. Mr. Geach has seen Malays when terrified turn pale and shake;
- and Mr. Brough Smyth states that a native Australian “being on one
- occasion much frightened, showed a complexion as nearly approaching to
- what we call paleness, as can well be conceived in the case of a very
- black man.” Mr. Dyson Lacy has seen extreme fear shown in an
- Australian, by a nervous twitching of the hands, feet, and lips; and by
- the perspiration standing on the skin. Many savages do not repress the
- signs of fear so much as Europeans; and they often tremble greatly.
- With the Kafir, Gaika says, in his rather quaint English, the shaking
- “of the body is much experienced, and the eyes are widely open.” With
- savages, the sphincter muscles are often relaxed, just as may be
- observed in much frightened dogs, and as I have seen with monkeys when
- terrified by being caught.
- _The erection of the hair_.—Some of the signs of fear deserve a little
- further consideration. Poets continually speak of the hair standing on
- end; Brutus says to the ghost of Caesar, “that mak’st my blood cold,
- and my hair to stare.” And Cardinal Beaufort, after the murder of
- Gloucester exclaims, “Comb down his hair; look, look, it stands
- upright.” As I did not feel sure whether writers of fiction might not
- have applied to man what they had often observed in animals, I begged
- for information from Dr. Crichton Browne with respect to the insane. He
- states in answer that he has repeatedly seen their hair erected under
- the influence of sudden and extreme terror. For instance, it is
- occasionally necessary to inject morphia, under the skin of an insane
- woman, who dreads the operation extremely, though it causes very little
- pain; for she believes that poison is being introduced into her system,
- and that her bones will be softened, and her flesh turned into dust.
- She becomes deadly pale; her limbs are stiffened by a sort of tetanic
- spasm, and her hair is partially erected on the front of the head.
- Dr. Browne further remarks that the bristling of the hair which is so
- common in the insane, is not always associated with terror. It is
- perhaps most frequently seen in chronic maniacs, who rave incoherently
- and have destructive impulses; but it is during their paroxysms of
- violence that the bristling is most observable. The fact of the hair
- becoming erect under the influence both of rage and fear agrees
- perfectly with what we have seen in the lower animals. Dr. Browne
- adduces several cases in evidence. Thus with a man now in the Asylum,
- before the recurrence of each maniacal paroxysm, “the hair rises up
- from his forehead like the mane of a Shetland pony.” He has sent me
- photographs of two women, taken in the intervals between their
- paroxysms, and he adds with respect to one of these women, “that the
- state of her hair is a sure and convenient criterion of her mental
- condition.” I have had one of these photographs copied, and the
- engraving gives, if viewed from a little distance, a faithful
- representation of the original, with the exception that the hair
- appears rather too coarse and too much curled. The extraordinary
- condition of the hair in the insane is due, not only to its erection,
- but to its dryness and harshness, consequent on the subcutaneous glands
- failing to act. Dr. Bucknill has said[1220] that a lunatic “is a
- lunatic to his finger’s ends;” he might have added, and often to the
- extremity of each particular hair.
- Dr. Browne mentions as an empirical confirmation of the relation which
- exists in the insane between the state of their hair and minds, that
- the wife of a medical man, who has charge of a lady suffering from
- acute melancholia, with a strong fear of death, for herself, her
- husband and children, reported verbally to him the day before receiving
- my letter as follows, “I think Mrs. —— will soon improve, for her hair
- is getting smooth; and I always notice that our patients get better
- whenever their hair ceases to be rough and unmanageable.”
- Dr. Browne attributes the persistently rough condition of the hair in
- many insane patients, in part to their minds being always somewhat
- disturbed, and in part to the effects of habit,—that is, to the hair
- being frequently and strongly erected during their many recurrent
- paroxysms. In patients in whom the bristling of the hair is extreme,
- the disease is generally permanent and mortal; but in others, in whom
- the bristling is moderate, as soon as they recover their health of mind
- the hair recovers its smoothness.
- In a previous chapter we have seen that with animals the hairs are
- erected by the contraction of minute, unstriped, and involuntary
- muscles, which run to each separate follicle. In addition to this
- action, Mr. J. Wood has clearly ascertained by experiment, as he
- informs me, that with man the hairs on the front of the head which
- slope forwards, and those on the back which slope backwards, are raised
- in opposite directions by the contraction of the occipito-frontalis or
- scalp muscle. So that this muscle seems to aid in the erection of the
- hairs on the head of man in the same manner as the homologous
- _panniculus carnosus_ aids, or takes the greater part, in the erection
- of the spines on the backs of some of the lower animals.
- _Contraction of the platysma myoides muscle_.—This muscle is spread
- over the sides of the neck, extending downwards to a little beneath the
- collar-bones, and upwards to the lower part of the cheeks. A portion,
- called the risorius, is represented in the woodcut (M) fig. 2. The
- contraction of this muscle draws the corners of the mouth and the lower
- parts of the checks downwards and backwards. It produces at the same
- time divergent, longitudinal, prominent ridges on the sides of the neck
- in the young; and, in old thin persons, fine transverse wrinkles. This
- muscle is sometimes said not to be under the control of the will; but
- almost every one, if told to draw the corners of his mouth backwards
- and downwards with great force, brings it into action. I have, however,
- heard of a man who can voluntarily act on it only on one side of his
- neck.
- Sir C. Bell[1221] and others have stated that this muscle is strongly
- contracted under the influence of fear; and Duchenne insists so
- strongly on its importance in the expression of this emotion, that he
- calls it the _muscle of fright_.[1222] He admits, however, that its
- contraction is quite inexpressive unless associated with widely open
- eyes and mouth. He has given a photograph (copied and reduced in the
- accompanying woodcut) of the same old man as on former occasions, with
- his eyebrows strongly raised, his mouth opened, and the platysma
- contracted, all by means of galvanism. The original photograph was
- shown to twenty-four persons, and they were separately asked, without
- any explanation being given, what expression was intended: twenty
- instantly answered, “intense fright” or “horror”; three said pain, and
- one extreme discomfort. Dr. Duchenne has given another photograph of
- the same old man, with the platysma contracted, the eyes and mouth
- opened, and the eyebrows rendered oblique, by means of galvanism. The
- expression thus induced is very striking (see Plate VII. fig. 2); the
- obliquity of the eyebrows adding the appearance of great mental
- distress. The original was shown to fifteen persons; twelve answered
- terror or horror, and three agony or great suffering. From these cases,
- and from an examination of the other photographs given by Dr. Duchenne,
- together with his remarks thereon, I think there can be little doubt
- that the contraction of the platysma does add greatly to the expression
- of fear. Nevertheless this muscle ought hardly to be called that of
- fright, for its contraction is certainly not a necessary concomitant of
- this state of mind.
- A man may exhibit extreme terror in the plainest manner by death-like
- pallor, by drops of perspiration on his skin, and by utter prostration,
- with all the muscles of his body, including the platysma, completely
- relaxed. Although Dr. Browne has often seen this muscle quivering and
- contracting in the insane, he has not been able to connect its action
- with any emotional condition in them, though he carefully attended to
- patients suffering from great fear. Mr. Nicol, on the other hand, has
- observed three cases in which this muscle appeared to be more or less
- permanently contracted under the influence of melancholia, associated
- with much dread; but in one of these cases, various other muscles about
- the neck and head were subject to spasmodic contractions.
- Dr. W. Ogle observed for me in one of the London hospitals about twenty
- patients, just before they were put under the influence of chloroform
- for operations. They exhibited some trepidation, but no great terror.
- In only four of the cases was the platysma visibly contracted; and it
- did not begin to contract until the patients began to cry. The muscle
- seemed to contract at the moment of each deep-drawn inspiration; so
- that it is very doubtful whether the contraction depended at all on the
- emotion of fear. In a fifth case, the patient, who was not
- chloroformed, was much terrified; and his platysma was more forcibly
- and persistently contracted than in the other cases. But even here
- there is room for doubt, for the muscle which appeared to be unusually
- developed, was seen by Dr. Ogle to contract as the man moved his head
- from the pillow, after the operation was over.
- As I felt much perplexed why, in any case, a superficial muscle on the
- neck should be especially affected by fear, I applied to my many
- obliging correspondents for information about the contraction of this
- muscle under other circumstances. It would be superfluous to give all
- the answers which I have received. They show that this muscle acts,
- often in a variable manner and degree, under many different conditions.
- It is violently contracted in hydrophobia, and in a somewhat less
- degree in lockjaw; sometimes in a marked manner during the
- insensibility from chloroform. Dr. W. Ogle observed two male patients,
- suffering from such difficulty in breathing, that the trachea had to be
- opened, and in both the platysma was strongly contracted. One of these
- men overheard the conversation of the surgeons surrounding him, and
- when he was able to speak, declared that he had not been frightened. In
- some other cases of extreme difficulty of respiration, though not
- requiring tracheotomy, observed by Drs. Ogle and Langstaff, the
- platysma was not contracted.
- Mr. J. Wood, who has studied with such care the muscles of the human
- body, as shown by his various publications, has often seen the platysma
- contracted in vomiting, nausea, and disgust; also in children and
- adults under the influence of rage,—for instance, in Irishwomen,
- quarrelling and brawling together with angry gesticulations. This may
- possibly have been due to their high and angry tones; for I know a
- lady, an excellent musician, who, in singing certain high notes, always
- contracts her platysma. So does a young man, as I have observed, in
- sounding certain notes on the flute. Mr. J. Wood informs me that he has
- found the platysma best developed in persons with thick necks and broad
- shoulders; and that in families inheriting these peculiarities, its
- development is usually associated with much voluntary power over the
- homologous occipito-frontalis muscle, by which the scalp can be moved.
- None of the foregoing cases appear to throw any light on the
- contraction of the platysma from fear; but it is different, I think,
- with the following cases. The gentleman before referred to, who can
- voluntarily act on this muscle only on one side of his neck, is
- positive that it contracts on both sides whenever he is startled.
- Evidence has already been given showing that this muscle sometimes
- contracts, perhaps for the sake of opening the mouth widely, when the
- breathing is rendered difficult by disease, and during the deep
- inspirations of crying-fits before an operation. Now, whenever a person
- starts at any sudden sight or sound, he instantaneously draws a deep
- breath; and thus the contraction of the platysma may possibly have
- become associated with the sense of fear. But there is, I believe, a
- more efficient relation. The first sensation of fear, or the
- imagination of something dreadful, commonly excites a shudder. I have
- caught myself giving a little involuntary shudder at a painful thought,
- and I distinctly perceived that my platysma contracted; so it does if I
- simulate a shudder. I have asked others to act in this manner; and in
- some the muscle contracted, but not in others. One of my sons, whilst
- getting out of bed, shuddered from the cold, and, as he happened to
- have his hand on his neck, he plainly felt that this muscle strongly
- contracted. He then voluntarily shuddered, as he had done on former
- occasions, but the platysma was not then affected. Mr. J. Wood has also
- several times observed this muscle contracting in patients, when
- stripped for examination, and who were not frightened, but shivered
- slightly from the cold. Unfortunately I have not been able to ascertain
- whether, when the whole body shakes, as in the cold stage of an ague
- fit, the platysma contracts. But as it certainly often contracts during
- a shudder; and as a shudder or shiver often accompanies the first
- sensation of fear, we have, I think, a clue to its action in this
- latter case.[1223] Its contraction, however, is not an invariable
- concomitant of fear; for it probably never acts under the influence of
- extreme, prostrating terror.
- _Dilatation of the Pupils_.—Gratiolet repeatedly insists[1224] that the
- pupils are enormously dilated whenever terror is felt. I have no reason
- to doubt the accuracy of this statement, but have failed to obtain
- confirmatory evidence, excepting in the one instance before given of an
- insane woman suffering from great fear. When writers of fiction speak
- of the eyes being widely dilated, I presume that they refer to the
- eyelids. Munro’s statement, that with parrots the iris is affected by
- the passions, independently of the amount of light, seems to bear on
- this question; but Professor Donders informs me, that he has often seen
- movements in the pupils of these birds which he thinks may be related
- to their power of accommodation to distance, in nearly the same manner
- as our own pupils contract when our eyes converge for near vision.
- Gratiolet remarks that the dilated pupils appear as if they were gazing
- into profound darkness. No doubt the fears of man have often been
- excited in the dark; but hardly so often or so exclusively, as to
- account for a fixed and associated habit having thus arisen. It seems
- more probable, assuming that Gratiolet’s statement is correct, that the
- brain is directly affected by the powerful emotion of fear and reacts
- on the pupils; but Professor Donders informs me that this is an
- extremely complicated subject. I may add, as possibly throwing light on
- the subject, that Dr. Fyffe, of Netley Hospital, has observed in two
- patients that the pupils were distinctly dilated during the cold stage
- of an ague fit. Professor Donders has also often seen dilatation of the
- pupils in incipient faintness.[1225]
- _Horror_.—The state of mind expressed by this term implies terror, and
- is in some, cases almost synonymous with it. Many a man must have felt,
- before the blessed discovery of chloroform, great horror at the thought
- of an impending surgical operation. He who dreads, as well as hates a
- man, will feel, as Milton uses the word, a horror of him. We feel
- horror if we see any one, for instance a child, exposed to some instant
- and crushing danger. Almost every one would experience the same feeling
- in the highest degree in witnessing a man being tortured or going to be
- tortured. In these cases there is no danger to ourselves; but from the
- power of the imagination and of sympathy we put ourselves in the
- position of the sufferer, and feel something akin to fear.
- Horror and Agony. Fig. 21
- Sir C. Bell remarks,[1226] that “horror is full of energy; the body is
- in the utmost tension, not unnerved by fear.” It is, therefore,
- probable that horror would generally be accompanied by the strong
- contraction of the brows; but as fear is one of the elements, the eyes
- and mouth would be opened, and the eyebrows would be raised, as far as
- the antagonistic action of the corrugators permitted this movement.
- Duchenne has given a photograph[1227] (fig. 21) of the same old man as
- before, with his eyes somewhat staring, the eyebrows partially raised,
- and at the same time strongly contracted, the mouth opened, and the
- platysma in action, all effected by the means of galvanism. He
- considers that the expression thus produced shows extreme terror with
- horrible pain or torture. A tortured man, as long as his sufferings
- allowed him to feel any dread for the future, would probably exhibit
- horror in an extreme degree. I have shown the original of this
- photograph to twenty-three persons of both sexes and various ages; and
- thirteen immediately answered horror, great pain, torture, or agony;
- three answered extreme fright; so that sixteen answered nearly in
- accordance with Duchenne’s belief. Six, however, said anger, guided no
- doubt, by the strongly contracted brows, and overlooking the peculiarly
- opened mouth. One said disgust. On the whole, the evidence indicates
- that we have here a fairly good representation of horror and agony. The
- photograph before referred to (Pl. VII. fig. 2) likewise exhibits
- horror; but in this the oblique eyebrows indicate great mental distress
- in place of energy.
- Horror is generally accompanied by various gestures, which differ in
- different individuals. Judging from pictures, the whole body is often
- turned away or shrinks; or the arms are violently protruded as if to
- push away some dreadful object. The most frequent gesture, as far as
- can be inferred from the action of persons who endeavour to express a
- vividly-imagined scene of horror, is the raising of both shoulders,
- with the bent arms pressed closely against the sides or chest. These
- movements are nearly the same with those commonly made when we feel
- very cold; and they are generally accompanied by a shudder, as well as
- by a deep expiration or inspiration, according as the chest happens at
- the time to be expanded or contracted. The sounds thus made are
- expressed by words like _uh_ or _ugh_.[1228] It is not, however,
- obvious why, when we feel cold or express a sense of horror, we press
- our bent arms against our bodies, raise our shoulders, and shudder.
- _Conclusion_.—I have now endeavoured to describe the diversified
- expressions of fear, in its gradations from mere attention to a start
- of surprise, into extreme terror and horror. Some of the signs may be
- accounted for through the principles of habit, association, and
- inheritance,—such as the wide opening of the mouth and eyes, with
- upraised eyebrows, so as to see as quickly as possible all around us,
- and to hear distinctly whatever sound may reach our ears. For we have
- thus habitually prepared ourselves to discover and encounter any
- danger. Some of the other signs of fear may likewise be accounted for,
- at least in part, through these same principles. Men, during numberless
- generations, have endeavoured to escape from their enemies or danger by
- headlong flight, or by violently struggling with them; and such great
- exertions will have caused the heart to beat rapidly, the breathing to
- be hurried, the chest to heave, and the nostrils to be dilated. As
- these exertions have often been prolonged to the last extremity, the
- final result will have been utter prostration, pallor, perspiration,
- trembling of all the muscles, or their complete relaxation. And now,
- whenever the emotion of fear is strongly felt, though it may not lead
- to any exertion, the same results tend to reappear, through the force
- of inheritance and association.
- Nevertheless, it is probable that many or most of the above symptoms of
- terror, such as the beating of the heart, the trembling of the muscles,
- cold perspiration, &c., are in large part directly due to the disturbed
- or interrupted transmission of nerve-force from the cerebro-spinal
- system to various parts of the body, owing to the mind being so
- powerfully affected. We may confidently look to this cause,
- independently of habit and association, in such cases as the modified
- secretions of the intestinal canal, and the failure of certain glands
- to act. With respect to the involuntary bristling of the hair, we have
- good reason to believe that in the case of animals this action, however
- it may have originated, serves, together with certain voluntary
- movements, to make them appear terrible to their enemies; and as the
- same involuntary and voluntary actions are performed by animals nearly
- related to man, we are led to believe that man has retained through
- inheritance a relic of them, now become useless. It is certainly a
- remarkable fact, that the minute unstriped muscles, by which the hairs
- thinly scattered over man’s almost naked body are erected, should have
- been preserved to the present day; and that they should still contract
- under the same emotions, namely, terror and rage, which cause the hairs
- to stand on end in the lower members of the Order to which man belongs.
- CHAPTER XIII. SELF-ATTENTION—SHAME—SHYNESS—MODESTY: BLUSHING.
- Nature of a blush—Inheritance—The parts of the body most
- affected—Blushing in the various races of man—Accompanying
- gestures—Confusion of mind—Causes of blushing—Self-attention, the
- fundamental element—Shyness—Shame, from broken moral laws and
- conventional rules—Modesty—Theory of blushing—Recapitulation.
- Blushing is the most peculiar and the most human of all expressions.
- Monkeys redden from passion, but it would require an overwhelming
- amount of evidence to make us believe that any animal could blush. The
- reddening of the face from a blush is due to the relaxation of the
- muscular coats of the small arteries, by which the capillaries become
- filled with blood; and this depends on the proper vaso-motor centre
- being affected. No doubt if there be at the same time much mental
- agitation, the general circulation will be affected; but it is not due
- to the action of the heart that the network of minute vessels covering
- the face becomes under a sense of shame gorged with blood. We can cause
- laughing by tickling the skin, weeping or frowning by a blow, trembling
- from the fear of pain, and so forth; but we cannot cause a blush, as
- Dr. Burgess remarks,[1301] by any physical means,—that is by any action
- on the body. It is the mind which must be affected. Blushing is not
- only involuntary; but the wish to restrain it, by leading to
- self-attention actually increases the tendency.
- The young blush much more freely than the old, but not during
- infancy,[1302] which is remarkable, as we know that infants at a very
- early age redden from passion. I have received authentic accounts of
- two little girls blushing at the ages of between two and three years;
- and of another sensitive child, a year older, blushing, when reproved
- for a fault. Many children, at a somewhat more advanced age blush in a
- strongly marked manner. It appears that the mental powers of infants
- are not as yet sufficiently developed to allow of their blushing.
- Hence, also, it is that idiots rarely blush. Dr. Crichton Browne
- observed for me those under his care, but never saw a genuine blush,
- though he has seen their faces flush, apparently from joy, when food
- was placed before them, and from anger. Nevertheless some, if not
- utterly degraded, are capable of blushing. A microcephalous idiot, for
- instance, thirteen years old, whose eyes brightened a little when he
- was pleased or amused, has been described by Dr. Behn,[1303] as
- blushing and turning to one side, when undressed for medical
- examination.
- Women blush much more than men. It is rare to see an old man, but not
- nearly so rare to see an old woman blushing. The blind do not escape.
- Laura Bridgman, born in this condition, as well as completely deaf,
- blushes.[1304] The Rev. R. H. Blair, Principal of the Worcester
- College, informs me that three children born blind, out of seven or
- eight then in the Asylum, are great blushers. The blind are not at
- first conscious that they are observed, and it is a most important part
- of their education, as Mr. Blair informs me, to impress this knowledge
- on their minds; and the impression thus gained would greatly strengthen
- the tendency to blush, by increasing the habit of self-attention.
- The tendency to blush is inherited. Dr. Burgess gives the case[1305] of
- a family consisting of a father, mother, and ten children, all of whom,
- without exception, were prone to blush to a most painful degree. The
- children were grown up; “and some of them were sent to travel in order
- to wear away this diseased sensibility, but nothing was of the
- slightest avail.” Even peculiarities in blushing seem to be inherited.
- Sir James Paget, whilst examining the spine of a girl, was struck at
- her singular manner of blushing; a big splash of red appeared first on
- one cheek, and then other splashes, variously scattered over the face
- and neck. He subsequently asked the mother whether her daughter always
- blushed in this peculiar manner; and was answered, “Yes, she takes
- after me.” Sir J. Paget then perceived that by asking this question he
- had caused the mother to blush; and she exhibited the same peculiarity
- as her daughter.
- In most cases the face, ears and neck are the sole parts which redden;
- but many persons, whilst blushing intensely, feel that their whole
- bodies grow hot and tingle; and this shows that the entire surface must
- be in some manner affected. Blushes are said sometimes to commence on
- the forehead, but more commonly on the cheeks, afterwards spreading to
- the ears and neck.[1306] In two Albinos examined by Dr. Burgess, the
- blushes commenced by a small circumscribed spot on the cheeks, over the
- parotidean plexus of nerves, and then increased into a circle; between
- this blushing circle and the blush on the neck there was an evident
- line of demarcation; although both arose simultaneously. The retina,
- which is naturally red in the Albino, invariably increased at the same
- time in redness.[1307] Every one must have noticed how easily after one
- blush fresh blushes chase each other over the face. Blushing is
- preceded by a peculiar sensation in the skin. According to Dr. Burgess
- the reddening of the skin is generally succeeded by a slight pallor,
- which shows that the capillary vessels contract after dilating. In some
- rare cases paleness instead of redness is caused under conditions which
- would naturally induce a blush. For instance, a young lady told me that
- in a large and crowded party she caught her hair so firmly on the
- button of a passing servant, that it took some time before she could be
- extricated; from her sensations she imagined that she had blushed
- crimson; but was assured by a friend that she had turned extremely
- pale.
- I was desirous to learn how far down the body blushes extend; and Sir
- J. Paget, who necessarily has frequent opportunities for observation,
- has kindly attended to this point for me during two or three years. He
- finds that with women who blush intensely on the face, ears, and nape
- of neck, the blush does not commonly extend any lower down the body. It
- is rare to see it as low down as the collar-bones and shoulder-blades;
- and he has never himself seen a single instance in which it extended
- below the upper part of the chest. He has also noticed that blushes
- sometimes die away downwards, not gradually and insensibly, but by
- irregular ruddy blotches. Dr. Langstaff has likewise observed for me
- several women whose bodies did not in the least redden while their
- faces were crimsoned with blushes. With the insane, some of whom appear
- to be particularly liable to blushing, Dr. J. Crichton Browne has
- several times seen the blush extend as far down as the collar-bones,
- and in two instances to the breasts. He gives me the case of a married
- woman, aged twenty-seven, who suffered from epilepsy. On the morning
- after her arrival in the Asylum, Dr. Browne, together with his
- assistants, visited her whilst she was in bed. The moment that he
- approached, she blushed deeply over her cheeks and temples; and the
- blush spread quickly to her ears. She was much agitated and tremulous.
- He unfastened the collar of her chemise in order to examine the state
- of her lungs; and then a brilliant blush rushed over her chest, in an
- arched line over the upper third of each breast, and extended downwards
- between the breasts nearly to the ensiform cartilage of the sternum.
- This case is interesting, as the blush did not thus extend downwards
- until it became intense by her attention being drawn to this part of
- her person. As the examination proceeded she became composed, and the
- blush disappeared; but on several subsequent occasions the same
- phenomena were observed.
- The foregoing facts show that, as a general rule, with English women,
- blushing does not extend beneath the neck and upper part of the chest.
- Nevertheless Sir J. Paget informs me that he has lately heard of a
- case, on which he can fully rely, in which a little girl, shocked by
- what she imagined to be an act of indelicacy, blushed all over her
- abdomen and the upper parts of her legs. Moreau also[1308] relates, on
- the authority of a celebrated painter, that the chest, shoulders, arms,
- and whole body of a girl, who unwillingly consented to serve as a
- model, reddened when she was first divested of her clothes.
- It is a rather curious question why, in most cases the face, ears, and
- neck alone redden, inasmuch as the whole surface of the body often
- tingles and grows hot. This seems to depend, chiefly, on the face and
- adjoining parts of the skin having been habitually exposed to the air,
- light, and alternations of temperature, by which the small arteries not
- only have acquired the habit of readily dilating and contracting, but
- appear to have become unusually developed in comparison with other
- parts of the surface.[1309] It is probably owing to this same cause, as
- M. Moreau and Dr. Burgess have remarked, that the face is so liable to
- redden under various circumstances, such as a fever-fit, ordinary heat,
- violent exertion, anger, a slight blow, &c.; and on the other hand that
- it is liable to grow pale from cold and fear, and to be discoloured
- during pregnancy. The face is also particularly liable to be affected
- by cutaneous complaints, by small-pox, erysipelas, &c. This view is
- likewise supported by the fact that the men of certain races, who
- habitually go nearly naked, often blush over their arms and chests and
- even down to their waists. A lady, who is a great blusher, informs Dr.
- Crichton Browne, that when she feels ashamed or is agitated, she
- blushes over her face, neck, wrists, and hands,—that is, over all the
- exposed portions of her skin. Nevertheless it may be doubted whether
- the habitual exposure of the skin of the face and neck, and its
- consequent power of reaction under stimulants of all kinds, is by
- itself sufficient to account for the much greater tendency in English
- women of these parts than of others to blush; for the hands are well
- supplied with nerves and small vessels, and have been as much exposed
- to the air as the face or neck, and yet the hands rarely blush. We
- shall presently see that the attention of the mind having been directed
- much more frequently and earnestly to the face than to any other part
- of the body, probably affords a sufficient explanation.
- _Blushing in the various races of man_.—The small vessels of the face
- become filled with blood, from the emotion of shame, in almost all the
- races of man, though in the very dark races no distinct change of
- colour can be perceived. Blushing is evident in all the Aryan nations
- of Europe, and to a certain extent with those of India. But Mr. Erskine
- has never noticed that the necks of the Hindoos are decidedly affected.
- With the Lepchas of Sikhim, Mr. Scott has often observed a faint blush
- on the cheeks, base of the ears, and sides of the neck, accompanied by
- sunken eyes and lowered head. This has occurred when he has detected
- them in a falsehood, or has accused them of ingratitude. The pale,
- sallow complexions of these men render a blush much more conspicuous
- than in most of the other natives of India. With the latter, shame, or
- it may be in part fear, is expressed, according to Mr. Scott, much more
- plainly by the head being averted or bent down, with the eyes wavering
- or turned askant, than by any change of colour in the skin.
- The Semitic races blush freely, as might have been expected, from their
- general similitude to the Aryans. Thus with the Jews, it is said in the
- Book of Jeremiah (chap. vi. 15), “Nay, they were not at all ashamed,
- neither could they blush.” Mrs. Asa Gray saw an Arab managing his boat
- clumsily on the Nile, and when laughed at by his companions, “he
- blushed quite to the back of his neck.” Lady Duff Gordon remarks that a
- young Arab blushed on coming into her presence.[1310]
- Mr. Swinhoe has seen the Chinese blushing, but he thinks it is rare;
- yet they have the expression “to redden with shame.” Mr. Geach informs
- me that the Chinese settled in Malacca and the native Malays of the
- interior both blush. Some of these people go nearly naked, and he
- particularly attended to the downward extension of the blush. Omitting
- the cases in which the face alone was seen to blush, Mr. Geach observed
- that the face, arms, and breast of a Chinaman, aged 24 years, reddened
- from shame; and with another Chinese, when asked why he had not done
- his work in better style, the whole body was similarly affected. In two
- Malays[1311] he saw the face, neck, breast, and arms blushing; and in a
- third Malay (a Bugis) the blush extended down to the waist.
- The Polynesians blush freely. The Rev. Mr. Stack has seen hundreds of
- instances with the New Zealanders. The following case is worth giving,
- as it relates to an old man who was unusually dark-coloured and partly
- tattooed. After having let his land to an Englishman for a small yearly
- rental, a strong passion seized him to buy a gig, which had lately
- become the fashion with the Maoris. He consequently wished to draw all
- the rent for four years from his tenant, and consulted Mr. Stack
- whether he could do so. The man was old, clumsy, poor, and ragged, and
- the idea of his driving himself about in his carriage for display
- amused Mr. Stack so much that he could not help bursting out into a
- laugh; and then “the old man blushed up to the roots of his hair.”
- Forster says that “you may easily distinguish a spreading blush” on the
- cheeks of the fairest women in Tahiti.[1312] The natives also of
- several of the other archipelagoes in the Pacific have been seen to
- blush.
- Mr. Washington Matthews has often seen a blush on the faces of the
- young squaws belonging to various wild Indian tribes of North America.
- At the opposite extremity of the continent in Tierra del Fuego, the
- natives, according to Mr. Bridges, “blush much, but chiefly in regard
- to women; but they certainly blush also at their own personal
- appearance.” This latter statement agrees with what I remember of the
- Fuegian, Jemmy Button, who blushed when he was quizzed about the care
- which he took in polishing his shoes, and in otherwise adorning
- himself. With respect to the Aymara Indians on the lofty plateaus of
- Bolivia, Mr. Forbes says,[1313] that from the colour of their skins it
- is impossible that their blushes should be as clearly visible as in the
- white races; still under such circumstances as would raise a blush in
- us, “there can always be seen the same expression of modesty or
- confusion; and even in the dark, a rise of temperature of the skin of
- the face can be felt, exactly as occurs in the European.” With the
- Indians who inhabit the hot, equable, and damp parts of South America,
- the skin apparently does not answer to mental excitement so readily as
- with the natives of the northern and southern parts of the continent,
- who have long been exposed to great vicissitudes of climate; for
- Humboldt quotes without a protest the sneer of the Spaniard, “How can
- those be trusted, who know not how to blush?”[1314] Von Spix and
- Martius, in speaking of the aborigines of Brazil, assert that they
- cannot properly be said to blush; “it was only after long intercourse
- with the whites, and after receiving some education, that we perceived
- in the Indians a change of colour expressive of the emotions of their
- minds.”[1315] It is, however, incredible that the power of blushing
- could have thus originated; but the habit of self-attention, consequent
- on their education and new course of life, would have much increased
- any innate tendency to blush.
- Several trustworthy observers have assured me that they have seen on
- the faces of negroes an appearance resembling a blush, under
- circumstances which would have excited one in us, though their skins
- were of an ebony-black tint. Some describe it as blushing brown, but
- most say that the blackness becomes more intense. An increased supply
- of blood in the skin seems in some manner to increase its blackness;
- thus certain exanthematous diseases cause the affected places in the
- negro to appear blacker, instead of, as with us, redder.[1316] The
- skin, perhaps, from being rendered more tense by the filling of the
- capillaries, would reflect a somewhat different tint to what it did
- before. That the capillaries of the face in the negro become filled
- with blood, under the emotion of shame, we may feel confident; because
- a perfectly characterized albino negress, described by Buffon,[1317]
- showed a faint tinge of crimson on her cheeks when she exhibited
- herself naked. Cicatrices of the skin remain for a long time white in
- the negro, and Dr. Burgess, who had frequent opportunities of observing
- a scar of this kind on the face of a negress, distinctly saw that it
- “invariably became red whenever she was abruptly spoken to, or charged
- with any trivial offence.”[1318] The blush could be seen proceeding
- from the circumference of the scar towards the middle, but it did not
- reach the centre. Mulattoes are often great blushers, blush succeeding
- blush over their faces. From these facts there can be no doubt that
- negroes blush, although no redness is visible on the skin.
- I am assured by Gaika and by Mrs. Barber that the Kafirs of South
- Africa never blush; but this may only mean that no change of colour is
- distinguishable. Gaika adds that under the circumstances which would
- make a European blush, his countrymen “look ashamed to keep their heads
- up.”
- It is asserted by four of my informants that the Australians, who are
- almost as black as negroes, never blush. A fifth answers doubtfully,
- remarking that only a very strong blush could be seen, on account of
- the dirty state of their skins. Three observers state that they do
- blush;[1319] Mr. S. Wilson adding that this is noticeable only under a
- strong emotion, and when the skin is not too dark from long exposure
- and want of cleanliness. Mr. Lang answers, “I have noticed that shame
- almost always excites a blush, which frequently extends as low as the
- neck.” Shame is also shown, as he adds, “by the eyes being turned from
- side to side.” As Mr. Lang was a teacher in a native school, it is
- probable that he chiefly observed children; and we know that they blush
- more than adults. Mr. G. Taplin has seen half-castes blushing, and he
- says that the aborigines have a word expressive of shame. Mr.
- Hagenauer, who is one of those who has never observed the Australians
- to blush, says that he has “seen them looking down to the ground on
- account of shame;” and the missionary, Mr. Bulmer, remarks that though
- “I have not been able to detect anything like shame in the adult
- aborigines, I have noticed that the eyes of the children, when ashamed,
- present a restless, watery appearance, as if they did not know where to
- look.”
- The facts now given are sufficient to show that blushing, whether or
- not there is any change of colour, is common to most, probably to all,
- of the races of man.
- _Movements and gestures which accompany Blushing_.—Under a keen sense
- of shame there is a strong desire for concealment.[1320] We turn away
- the whole body, more especially the face, which we endeavour in some
- manner to hide. An ashamed person can hardly endure to meet the gaze of
- those present, so that he almost invariably casts down his eyes or
- looks askant. As there generally exists at the same time a strong wish
- to avoid the appearance of shame, a vain attempt is made to look direct
- at the person who causes this feeling; and the antagonism between these
- opposite tendencies leads to various restless movements in the eyes. I
- have noticed two ladies who, whilst blushing, to which they are very
- liable, have thus acquired, as it appears, the oddest trick of
- incessantly blinking their eyelids with extraordinary rapidity. An
- intense blush is sometimes accompanied by a slight effusion of
- tears;[1321] and this, I presume, is due to the lacrymal glands
- partaking of the increased supply of blood, which we know rushes into
- the capillaries of the adjoining parts, including the retina.
- Many writers, ancient and modern, have noticed the foregoing movements;
- and it has already been shown that the aborigines in various parts of
- the world often exhibit their shame by looking downwards or askant, or
- by restless movements of their eyes. Ezra cries out (ch. ix. 6), “O, my
- God! I am ashamed, and blush to lift up my head to thee, my God.” In
- Isaiah (ch. I. 6) we meet with the words, “I hid not my face from
- shame.” Seneca remarks (Epist. xi. 5) “that the Roman players hang down
- their heads, fix their eyes on the ground and keep them lowered, but
- are unable to blush in acting shame.” According to Macrobius, who lived
- in the filth century (‘Saturnalia,’ B. vii. C. 11), “Natural
- philosophers assert that nature being moved by shame spreads the blood
- before herself as a veil, as we see any one blushing often puts his
- hands before his face.” Shakspeare makes Marcus (‘Titus Andronicus,’
- act ii, sc. 5) say to his niece, “Ah! now thou turn’st away thy face
- for shame.” A lady informs me that she found in the Lock Hospital a
- girl whom she had formerly known, and who had become a wretched
- castaway, and the poor creature, when approached, hid her face under
- the bed-clothes, and could not be persuaded to uncover it. We often see
- little children, when shy or ashamed, turn away, and still standing up,
- bury their faces in their mother’s gown; or they throw themselves face
- downwards on her lap.
- _Confusion of mind_.—Most persons, whilst blushing intensely, have
- their mental powers confused. This is recognized in such common
- expressions as “she was covered with confusion.” Persons in this
- condition lose their presence of mind, and utter singularly
- inappropriate remarks. They are often much distressed, stammer, and
- make awkward movements or strange grimaces. In certain cases
- involuntary twitchings of some of the facial muscles may be observed. I
- have been informed by a young lady, who blushes excessively, that at
- such times she does not even know what she is saying. When it was
- suggested to her that this might be due to her distress from the
- consciousness that her blushing was noticed, she answered that this
- could not be the case, “as she had sometimes felt quite as stupid when
- blushing at a thought in her own room.”
- I will give an instance of the extreme disturbance of mind to which
- some sensitive men are liable. A gentleman, on whom I can rely, assured
- me that he had been an eye-witness of the following scene:—A small
- dinner-party was given in honour of an extremely shy man, who, when he
- rose to return thanks, rehearsed the speech, which he had evidently
- learnt by heart, in absolute silence, and did not utter a single word;
- but he acted as if he were speaking with much emphasis. His friends,
- perceiving how the case stood, loudly applauded the imaginary bursts of
- eloquence, whenever his gestures indicated a pause, and the man never
- discovered that he had remained the whole time completely silent. On
- the contrary, he afterwards remarked to my friend, with much
- satisfaction, that he thought he had succeeded uncommonly well.
- When a person is much ashamed or very shy, and blushes intensely, his
- heart beats rapidly and his breathing is disturbed. This can hardly
- fail to affect the circulation of the blood within the brain, and
- perhaps the mental powers. It seems however doubtful, judging from the
- still more powerful influence of anger and fear on the circulation,
- whether we can thus satisfactorily account for the confused state of
- mind in persons whilst blushing intensely.
- The true explanation apparently lies in the intimate sympathy which
- exists between the capillary circulation of the surface of the head and
- face, and that of the brain. On applying to Dr. J. Crichton Browne for
- information, he has given me various facts bearing on this subject.
- When the sympathetic nerve is divided on one side of the head, the
- capillaries on this side are relaxed and become filled with blood,
- causing the skin to redden and to grow hot, and at the same time the
- temperature within the cranium on the same side rises. Inflammation of
- the membranes of the brain leads to the engorgement of the face, ears,
- and eyes with blood. The first stage of an epileptic fit appears to be
- the contraction of the vessels of the brain, and the first outward
- manifestation is, an extreme pallor of countenance. Erysipelas of the
- head commonly induces delirium. Even the relief given to a severe
- headache by burning the skin with strong lotion, depends, I presume, on
- the same principle.
- Dr. Browne has often administered to his patients the vapour of the
- nitrite of amyl,[1322] which has the singular property of causing vivid
- redness of the face in from thirty to sixty seconds. This flushing
- resembles blushing in almost every detail: it begins at several
- distinct points on the face, and spreads till it involves the whole
- surface of the head, neck, and front of the chest; but has been
- observed to extend only in one case to the abdomen. The arteries in the
- retina become enlarged; the eyes glisten, and in one instance there was
- a slight effusion of tears. The patients are at first pleasantly
- stimulated, but, as the flushing increases, they become confused and
- bewildered. One woman to whom the vapour had often been administered
- asserted that, as soon as she grew hot, she grew MUDDLED. With persons
- just commencing to blush it appears, judging from their bright eyes and
- lively behaviour, that their mental powers are somewhat stimulated. It
- is only when the blushing is excessive that the mind grows confused.
- Therefore it would seem that the capillaries of the face are affected,
- both during the inhalation of the nitrite of amyl and during blushing,
- before that part of the brain is affected on which the mental powers
- depend.
- Conversely when the brain is primarily affected; the circulation of the
- skin is so in a secondary manner. Dr. Browne has frequently observed,
- as he informs me, scattered red blotches and mottlings on the chests of
- epileptic patients. In these cases, when the skin on the thorax or
- abdomen is gently rubbed with a pencil or other object, or, in
- strongly-marked cases, is merely touched by the finger, the surface
- becomes suffused in less than half a minute with bright red marks,
- which spread to some distance on each side of the touched point, and
- persist for several minutes. These are the _cerebral maculae_ of
- Trousseau; and they indicate, as Dr. Browne remarks, a highly modified
- condition of the cutaneous vascular system. If, then, there exists, as
- cannot be doubted, an intimate sympathy between the capillary
- circulation in that part of the brain on which our mental powers
- depend, and in the skin of the face, it is not surprising that the
- moral causes which induce intense blushing should likewise induce,
- independently of their own disturbing influence, much confusion of
- mind.
- _The Nature of the Mental States which induce Blushing_.—These consist
- of shyness, shame, and modesty; the essential element in all being
- self-attention. Many reasons can be assigned for believing that
- originally self-attention directed to personal appearance, in relation
- to the opinion of others, was the exciting cause; the same effect being
- subsequently produced, through the force of association, by
- self-attention in relation to moral conduct. It is not the simple act
- of reflecting on our own appearance, but the thinking what others think
- of us, which excites a blush. In absolute solitude the most sensitive
- person would be quite indifferent about his appearance. We feel blame
- or disapprobation more acutely than approbation; and consequently
- depreciatory remarks or ridicule, whether of our appearance or conduct,
- causes us to blush much more readily than does praise. But undoubtedly
- praise and admiration are highly efficient: a pretty girl blushes when
- a man gazes intently at her, though she may know perfectly well that he
- is not depreciating her. Many children, as well as old and sensitive
- persons blush, when they are much praised. Hereafter the question will
- be discussed, how it has arisen that the consciousness that others are
- attending to our personal appearance should have led to the
- capillaries, especially those of the face, instantly becoming filled
- with blood.
- My reasons for believing that attention directed to personal
- appearance, and not to moral conduct, has been the fundamental element
- in the acquirement of the habit of blushing, will now be given. They
- are separately light, but combined possess, as it appears to me,
- considerable weight. It is notorious that nothing makes a shy person
- blush so much as any remark, however slight, on his personal
- appearance. One cannot notice even the dress of a woman much given to
- blushing, without causing her face to crimson. It is sufficient to
- stare hard at some persons to make them, as Coleridge remarks,
- blush,—“account for that he who can.”[1323]
- With the two albinos observed by Dr. Burgess,[1324] “the slightest
- attempt to examine their peculiarities invariably caused them to blush
- deeply.” Women are much more sensitive about their personal appearance
- than men are, especially elderly women in comparison with elderly men,
- and they blush much more freely. The young of both sexes are much more
- sensitive on this same head than the old, and they also blush much more
- freely than the old. Children at a very early age do not blush; nor do
- they show those other signs of self-consciousness which generally
- accompany blushing; and it is one of their chief charms that they think
- nothing about what others think of them. At this early age they will
- stare at a stranger with a fixed gaze and un-blinking eyes, as on an
- inanimate object, in a manner which we elders cannot imitate.
- It is plain to every one that young men and women are highly sensitive
- to the opinion of each other with reference to their personal
- appearance; and they blush incomparably more in the presence of the
- opposite sex than in that of their own.[1325] A young man, not very
- liable to blush, will blush intensely at any slight ridicule of his
- appearance from a girl whose judgment on any important subject he would
- disregard. No happy pair of young lovers, valuing each other’s
- admiration and love more than anything else in the world, probably ever
- courted each other without many a blush. Even the barbarians of Tierra
- del Fuego, according to Mr. Bridges, blush “chiefly in regard to women,
- but certainly also at their own personal appearance.”
- Of all parts of the body, the face is most considered and regarded, as
- is natural from its being the chief seat of expression and the source
- of the voice. It is also the chief seat of beauty and of ugliness, and
- throughout the world is the most ornamented.[1326] The face, therefore,
- will have been subjected during many generations to much closer and
- more earnest self-attention than any other part of the body; and in
- accordance with the principle here advanced we can understand why it
- should be the most liable to blush. Although exposure to alternations
- of temperature, &c., has probably much increased the power of
- dilatation and contraction in the capillaries of the face and adjoining
- parts, yet this by itself will hardly account for these parts blushing
- much more than the rest of the body; for it does not explain the fact
- of the hands rarely blushing. With Europeans the whole body tingles
- slightly when the face blushes intensely; and with the races of men who
- habitually go nearly naked, the blushes extend over a much larger
- surface than with us. These facts are, to a certain extent,
- intelligible, as the self-attention of primeval man, as well as of the
- existing races which still go naked, will not have been so exclusively
- confined to their faces, as is the case with the people who now go
- clothed.
- We have seen that in all parts of the world persons who feel shame for
- some moral delinquency, are apt to avert, bend down, or hide their
- faces, independently of any thought about their personal appearance.
- The object can hardly be to conceal their blushes, for the face is thus
- averted or hidden under circumstances which exclude any desire to
- conceal shame, as when guilt is fully confessed and repented of. It is,
- however, probable that primeval man before he had acquired much moral
- sensitiveness would have been highly sensitive about his personal
- appearance, at least in reference to the other sex, and he would
- consequently have felt distress at any depreciatory remarks about his
- appearance; and this is one form of shame. And as the face is the part
- of the body which is most regarded, it is intelligible that any one
- ashamed of his personal appearance would desire to conceal this part of
- his body. The habit having been thus acquired, would naturally be
- carried on when shame from strictly moral causes was felt; and it is
- not easy otherwise to see why under these circumstances there should be
- a desire to hide the face more than any other part of the body.
- The habit, so general with every one who feels ashamed, of turning
- away, or lowering his eyes, or restlessly moving them from side to
- side, probably follows from each glance directed towards those present,
- bringing home the conviction that he is intently regarded; and he
- endeavours, by not looking at those present, and especially not at
- their eyes, momentarily to escape from this painful conviction.
- _Shyness_.—This odd state of mind, often called shamefacedness, or
- false shame, or _mauvaise honte_, appears to be one of the most
- efficient of all the causes of blushing. Shyness is, indeed, chiefly
- recognized by the face reddening, by the eyes being averted or cast
- down, and by awkward, nervous movements of the body. Many a woman
- blushes from this cause, a hundred, perhaps a thousand times, to once
- that she blushes from having done anything deserving blame, and of
- which she is truly ashamed. Shyness seems to depend on sensitiveness to
- the opinion, whether good or bad, of others, more especially with
- respect to external appearance. Strangers neither know nor care
- anything about our conduct or character, but they may, and often do,
- criticize our appearance: hence shy persons are particularly apt to be
- shy and to blush in the presence of strangers. The consciousness of
- anything peculiar, or even new, in the dress, or any slight blemish on
- the person, and more especially, on the face—points which are likely to
- attract the attention of strangers—makes the shy intolerably shy. On
- the other hand, in those cases in which conduct and not personal
- appearance is concerned, we are much more apt to be shy in the presence
- of acquaintances, whose judgment we in some degree value, than in that
- of strangers. A physician told me that a young man, a wealthy duke,
- with whom he had travelled as medical attendant, blushed like a girl,
- when he paid him his fee; yet this young man probably would not have
- blushed and been shy, had he been paying a bill to a tradesman. Some
- persons, however, are so sensitive, that the mere act of speaking to
- almost any one is sufficient to rouse their self-consciousness, and a
- slight blush is the result.
- Disapprobation or ridicule, from our sensitiveness on this head, causes
- shyness and blushing much more readily than does approbation; though
- the latter with some persons is highly efficient. The conceited are
- rarely shy; for they value themselves much too highly to expect
- depreciation. Why a proud man is often shy, as appears to be the case,
- is not so obvious, unless it be that, with all his self-reliance, he
- really thinks much about the opinion of others although in a disdainful
- spirit. Persons who are exceedingly shy are rarely shy in the presence
- of those with whom they are quite familiar, and of whose good opinion
- and sympathy they are perfectly assured;—for instance, a girl in the
- presence of her mother. I neglected to inquire in my printed paper
- whether shyness can be detected in the different races of man; but a
- Hindoo gentleman assured Mr. Erskine that it is recognizable in his
- countrymen.
- Shyness, as the derivation of the word indicates in several
- languages,[1327] is closely related to fear; yet it is distinct from
- fear in the ordinary sense. A shy man no doubt dreads the notice of
- strangers, but can hardly be said to be afraid of them, he may be as
- bold as a hero in battle, and yet have no self-confidence about trifles
- in the presence of strangers. Almost every one is extremely nervous
- when first addressing a public assembly, and most men remain so
- throughout their lives; but this appears to depend on the consciousness
- of a great coming exertion, with its associated effects on the system,
- rather than on shyness;[1328] although a timid or shy man no doubt
- suffers on such occasions infinitely more than another. With very young
- children it is difficult to distinguish between fear and shyness; but
- this latter feeling with them has often seemed to me to partake of the
- character of the wildness of an untamed animal. Shyness comes on at a
- very early age. In one of my own children, when two years and three
- months old, I saw a trace of what certainly appeared to be shyness,
- directed towards myself after an absence from home of only a week. This
- was shown not by a blush, but by the eyes being for a few minutes
- slightly averted from me. I have noticed on other occasions that
- shyness or shamefacedness and real shame are exhibited in the eyes of
- young children before they have acquired the power of blushing.
- As shyness apparently depends on self-attention, we can perceive how
- right are those who maintain that reprehending children for shyness,
- instead of doing them any good, does much harm, as it calls their
- attention still more closely to themselves. It has been well urged that
- “nothing hurts young people more than to be watched continually about
- their feelings, to have their countenances scrutinized, and the degrees
- of their sensibility measured by the surveying eye of the unmerciful
- spectator. Under the constraint of such examinations they can think of
- nothing but that they are looked at, and feel nothing but shame or
- apprehension.”[1329]
- _Moral causes: guilt_.—With respect to blushing from strictly moral
- causes, we meet with the same fundamental principle as before, namely,
- regard for the opinion of others. It is not the conscience which raises
- a blush, for a man may sincerely regret some slight fault committed in
- solitude, or he may suffer the deepest remorse for an undetected crime,
- but he will not blush. “I blush,” says Dr. Burgess,[1330] “in the
- presence of my accusers.” It is not the sense of guilt, but the thought
- that others think or know us to be guilty which crimsons the face. A
- man may feel thoroughly ashamed at having told a small falsehood,
- without blushing; but if he even suspects that he is detected he will
- instantly blush, especially if detected by one whom he reveres.
- On the other hand, a man may be convinced that God witnesses all his
- actions, and he may feel deeply conscious of some fault and pray for
- forgiveness; but this will not, as a lady who is a great blusher
- believes, ever excite a blush. The explanation of this difference
- between the knowledge by God and man of our actions lies, I presume, in
- man’s disapprobation of immoral conduct being somewhat akin in nature
- to his depreciation of our personal appearance, so that through
- association both lead to similar results; whereas the disapprobation of
- God brings up no such association.
- Many a person has blushed intensely when accused of some crime, though
- completely innocent of it. Even the thought, as the lady before
- referred to has observed to me, that others think that we have made an
- unkind or stupid remark, is amply sufficient to cause a blush, although
- we know all the time that we have been completely misunderstood. An
- action may be meritorious or of an indifferent nature, but a sensitive
- person, if he suspects that others take a different view of it, will
- blush. For instance, a lady by herself may give money to a beggar
- without a trace of a blush, but if others are present, and she doubts
- whether they approve, or suspects that they think her influenced by
- display, she will blush. So it will be, if she offers to relieve the
- distress of a decayed gentlewoman, more particularly of one whom she
- had previously known under better circumstances, as she cannot then
- feel sure how her conduct will be viewed. But such cases as these blend
- into shyness.
- _Breaches of etiquette_.—The rules of _etiquette_ always refer to
- conduct in the presence of, or towards others. They have no necessary
- connection with the moral sense, and are often meaningless.
- Nevertheless as they depend on the fixed custom of our equals and
- superiors, whose opinion we highly regard, they are considered almost
- as binding as are the laws of honour to a gentleman. Consequently the
- breach of the laws of etiquette, that is, any impoliteness or
- _gaucherie_, any impropriety, or an inappropriate remark, though quite
- accidental, will cause the most intense blushing of which a man is
- capable. Even the recollection of such an act, after an interval of
- many years, will make the whole body to tingle. So strong, also, is the
- power of sympathy that a sensitive person, as a lady has assured me,
- will sometimes blush at a flagrant breach of etiquette by a perfect
- stranger, though the act may in no way concern her.
- _Modesty_.—This is another powerful agent in exciting blushes; but the
- word modesty includes very different states of the mind. It implies
- humility, and we often judge of this by persons being greatly pleased
- and blushing at slight praise, or by being annoyed at praise which
- seems to them too high according to their own humble standard of
- themselves. Blushing here has the usual signification of regard for the
- opinion of others. But modesty frequently relates to acts of
- indelicacy; and indelicacy is an affair of etiquette, as we clearly see
- with the nations that go altogether or nearly naked. He who is modest,
- and blushes easily at acts of this nature, does so because they are
- breaches of a firmly and wisely established etiquette. This is indeed
- shown by the derivation of the word _modest_ from _modus_, a measure or
- standard of behaviour. A blush due to this form of modesty is,
- moreover, apt to be intense, because it generally relates to the
- opposite sex; and we have seen how in all cases our liability to blush
- is thus increased. We apply the term ‘modest,’ as it would appear, to
- those who have an humble opinion of themselves, and to those who are
- extremely sensitive about an indelicate word or deed, simply because in
- both cases blushes are readily excited, for these two frames of mind
- have nothing else in common. Shyness also, from this same cause, is
- often mistaken for modesty in the sense of humility.
- Some persons flush up, as I have observed and have been assured, at any
- sudden and disagreeable recollection. The commonest cause seems to be
- the sudden remembrance of not having done something for another person
- which had been promised. In this case it may be that the thought passes
- half unconsciously through the mind, “What will he think of me?” and
- then the flush would partake of the nature of a true blush. But whether
- such flushes are in most cases due to the capillary circulation being
- affected, is very doubtful; for we must remember that almost every
- strong emotion, such as anger or great joy, acts on the heart, and
- causes the face to redden.
- The fact that blushes may be excited in absolute solitude seems opposed
- to the view here taken, namely that the habit originally arose from
- thinking about what others think of us. Several ladies, who are great
- blushers, are unanimous in regard to solitude; and some of them believe
- that they have blushed in the dark. From what Mr. Forbes has stated
- with respect to the Aymaras, and from my own sensations, I have no
- doubt that this latter statement is correct. Shakspeare, therefore,
- erred when he made Juliet, who was not even by herself, say to Romeo
- (act ii. sc. 2):—
- “Thou know’st the mask of night is on my face;
- Else would a maiden blush bepaint my cheek,
- For that which thou hast heard me speak to-night.”
- But when a blush is excited in solitude, the cause almost always
- relates to the thoughts of others about us—to acts done in their
- presence, or suspected by them; or again when we reflect what others
- would have thought of us had they known of the act. Nevertheless one or
- two of my informants believe that they have blushed from shame at acts
- in no way relating to others. If this be so, we must attribute the
- result to the force of inveterate habit and association, under a state
- of mind closely analogous to that which ordinarily excites a blush; nor
- need we feel surprise at this, as even sympathy with another person who
- commits a flagrant breach of etiquette is believed, as we have just
- seen, sometimes to cause a blush.
- Finally, then, I conclude that blushing,—whether due to shyness—to
- shame for a real crime—to shame from a breach of the laws of
- etiquette—to modesty from humility—to modesty from an
- indelicacy—depends in all cases on the same principle; this principle
- being a sensitive regard for the opinion, more particularly for the
- depreciation of others, primarily in relation to our personal
- appearance, especially of our faces; and secondarily, through the force
- of association and habit, in relation to the opinion of others on our
- conduct.
- _Theory of Blushing_.—We have now to consider, why should the thought
- that others are thinking about us affect our capillary circulation? Sir
- C. Bell insists[1331] that blushing “is a provision for expression, as
- may be inferred from the colour extending only to the surface of the
- face, neck, and breast, the parts most exposed. It is not acquired; it
- is from the beginning.” Dr. Burgess believes that it was designed by
- the Creator in “order that the soul might have sovereign power of
- displaying in the cheeks the various internal emotions of the moral
- feelings;” so as to serve as a check on ourselves, and as a sign to
- others, that we were violating rules which ought to be held sacred.
- Gratiolet merely remarks,—“Or, comme il est dans l’ordre de la nature
- que l’être social le plus intelligent soit aussi le plus intelligible,
- cette faculté de rougeur et de pâleur qui distingue l’homme, est un
- signe naturel de sa haute perfection.”
- The belief that blushing was SPECIALLY designed by the Creator is
- opposed to the general theory of evolution, which is now so largely
- accepted; but it forms no part of my duty here to argue on the general
- question. Those who believe in design, will find it difficult to
- account for shyness being the most frequent and efficient of all the
- causes of blushing, as it makes the blusher to suffer and the beholder
- uncomfortable, without being of the least service to either of them.
- They will also find it difficult to account for negroes and other
- dark-coloured races blushing, in whom a change of colour in the skin is
- scarcely or not at all visible.
- No doubt a slight blush adds to the beauty of a maiden’s face; and the
- Circassian women who are capable of blushing, invariably fetch a higher
- price in the seraolio of the Sultan than less susceptible women.[1332]
- But the firmest believer in the efficacy of sexual selection will
- hardly suppose that blushing was acquired as a sexual ornament. This
- view would also be opposed to what has just been said about the
- dark-coloured races blushing in an invisible manner.
- The hypothesis which appears to me the most probable, though it may at
- first seem rash, is that attention closely directed to any part of the
- body tends to interfere with the ordinary and tonic contraction of the
- small arteries of that part. These vessels, in consequence, become at
- such times more or less relaxed, and are instantly filled with arterial
- blood. This tendency will have been much strengthened, if frequent
- attention has been paid during many generations to the same part, owing
- to nerve-force readily flowing along accustomed channels, and by the
- power of inheritance. Whenever we believe that others are depreciating
- or even considering our personal appearance, our attention is vividly
- directed to the outer and visible parts of our bodies; and of all such
- parts we are most sensitive about our faces, as no doubt has been the
- case during many past generations. Therefore, assuming for the moment
- that the capillary vessels can be acted on by close attention, those of
- the face will have become eminently susceptible. Through the force of
- association, the same effects will tend to follow whenever we think
- that others are considering or censuring our actions or character.
- As the basis of this theory rests on mental attention having some power
- to influence the capillary circulation, it will be necessary to give a
- considerable body of details, bearing more or less directly on this
- subject. Several observers,[1333] who from their wide experience and
- knowledge are eminently capable of forming a sound judgment, are
- convinced that attention or consciousness (which latter term Sir H.
- Holland thinks the more explicit) concentrated on almost any part of
- the body produces some direct physical effect on it. This applies to
- the movements of the involuntary muscles, and of the voluntary muscles
- when acting involuntarily,—to the secretion of the glands,—to the
- activity of the senses and sensations,—and even to the nutrition of
- parts.
- It is known that the involuntary movements of the heart are affected if
- close attention be paid to them. Gratiolet[1334] gives the case of a
- man, who by continually watching and counting his own pulse, at last
- caused one beat out of every six to intermit. On the other hand, my
- father told me of a careful observer, who certainly had heart-disease
- and died from it, and who positively stated that his pulse was
- habitually irregular to an extreme degree; yet to his great
- disappointment it invariably became regular as soon as my father
- entered the room. Sir H. Holland remarks, that “the effect upon the
- circulation of a part from the consciousness suddenly directed and
- fixed upon it, is often obvious and immediate.” Professor Laycock, who
- has particularly attended to phenomena of this nature, insists that
- “when the attention is directed to any portion of the body, innervation
- and circulation are excited locally, and the functional activity of
- that portion developed.”
- It is generally believed that the peristaltic movements of the
- intestines are influenced by attention being paid to them at fixed
- recurrent periods; and these movements depend on the contraction of
- unstriped and involuntary muscles. The abnormal action of the voluntary
- muscles in epilepsy, chorea, and hysteria is known to be influenced by
- the expectation of an attack, and by the sight of other patients
- similarly affected. So it is with the involuntary acts of yawning and
- laughing.
- Certain glands are much influenced by thinking of them, or of the
- conditions under which they have been habitually excited. This is
- familiar to every one in the increased flow of saliva, when the
- thought, for instance, of intensely acid fruit is kept before the mind.
- It was shown in our sixth chapter, that an earnest and long-continued
- desire either to repress, or to increase, the action of the lacrymal
- glands is effectual. Some curious cases have been recorded in the case
- of women, of the power of the mind on the mammary glands; and still
- more remarkable ones in relation to the uterine functions.
- See Gratiolet on this subject, De la Phys. p. 287. Dr. J. Crichton
- Browne, from his observations on the insane, is convinced that
- attention directed for a prolonged period on any part or organ may
- ultimately influence its capillary circulation and nutrition. He has
- given me some extraordinary cases; one of these, which cannot here be
- related in full, refers to a married woman fifty years of age, who
- laboured under the firm and long-continued delusion that she was
- pregnant. When the expected period arrived, she acted precisely as if
- she had been really delivered of a child, and seemed to suffer extreme
- pain, so that the perspiration broke out on her forehead. The result
- was that a state of things returned, continuing for three days, which
- had ceased during the six previous years. Mr. Braid gives, in his
- ‘Magic, Hypnotism,’ &c., 1852, p. 95, and in his other works analogous
- cases, as well as other facts showing the great influence of the will
- on the mammary glands, even on one breast alone.
- When we direct our whole attention to any one sense, its acuteness is
- increased;[1340] and the continued habit of close attention, as with
- blind people to that of hearing, and with the blind and deaf to that of
- touch, appears to improve the sense in question permanently. There is,
- also, some reason to believe, judging from the capacities of different
- races of man, that the effects are inherited. Turning to ordinary
- sensations, it is well known that pain is increased by attending to it;
- and Sir B. Brodie goes so far as to believe that pain may be felt in
- any part of the body to which attention is closely drawn.[1341] Sir H.
- Holland also remarks that we become not only conscious of the existence
- of a part subjected to concentrated attention, but we experience in it
- various odd sensations as of weight, heat, cold, tingling, or
- itching.[1342]
- Lastly, some physiologists maintain that the mind can influence the
- nutrition of parts. Sir J. Paget has given a curious instance of the
- power, not indeed of the mind, but of the nervous system, on the hair.
- A lady “who is subject to attacks of what is called nervous headache,
- always finds in the morning after such an one, that some patches of her
- hair are white, as if powdered with starch. The change is effected in a
- night, and in a few days after, the hairs gradually regain their dark
- brownish colour.”[1343]
- We thus see that close attention certainly affects various parts and
- organs, which are not properly under the control of the will. By what
- means attention—perhaps the most wonderful of all the wondrous powers
- of the mind—is effected, is an extremely obscure subject. According to
- Müller,[1344] the process by which the sensory cells of the brain are
- rendered, through the will, susceptible of receiving more intense and
- distinct impressions, is closely analogous to that by which the motor
- cells are excited to send nerve-force to the voluntary muscles. There
- are many points of analogy in the action of the sensory and motor
- nerve-cells; for instance, the familiar fact that close attention to
- any one sense causes fatigue, like the prolonged exertion of any one
- muscle.[1345] When therefore we voluntarily concentrate our attention
- on any part of the body, the cells of the brain which receive
- impressions or sensations from that part are, it is probable, in some
- unknown manner stimulated into activity. This may account, without any
- local change in the part to which our attention is earnestly directed,
- for pain or odd sensations being there felt or increased.
- If, however, the part is furnished with muscles, we cannot feel sure,
- as Mr. Michael Foster has remarked to me, that some slight impulse may
- not be unconsciously sent to such muscles; and this would probably
- cause an obscure sensation in the part.
- In a large number of cases, as with the salivary and lacrymal glands,
- intestinal canal, &c., the power of attention seems to rest, either
- chiefly, or as some physiologists think, exclusively, on the vaso-motor
- system being affected in such a manner that more blood is allowed to
- flow into the capillaries of the part in question. This increased
- action of the capillaries may in some cases be combined with the
- simultaneously increased activity of the sensorium.
- The manner in which the mind affects the vasomotor system may be
- conceived in the following manner. When we actually taste sour fruit,
- an impression is sent through the gustatory nerves to a certain part of
- the sensorium; this transmits nerve-force to the vasomotor centre,
- which consequently allows the muscular coats of the small arteries that
- permeate the salivary glands to relax. Hence more blood flows into
- these glands, and they secrete a copious supply of saliva. Now it does
- not seem an improbable assumption, that, when we reflect intently on a
- sensation, the same part of the sensorium, or a closely connected part
- of it, is brought into a state of activity, in the same manner as when
- we actually perceive the sensation. If so, the same cells in the brain
- will be excited, though, perhaps, in a less degree, by vividly thinking
- about a sour taste, as by perceiving it; and they will transmit in the
- one case, as in the other, nerve-force to the vaso-motor centre with
- the same results.
- To give another, and, in some respects, more appropriate illustration.
- If a man stands before a hot fire, his face reddens. This appears to be
- due, as Mr. Michael Foster informs me, in part to the local action of
- the heat, and in part to a reflex action from the vaso-motor
- centres.[1346] In this latter case, the heat affects the nerves of the
- face; these transmit an impression to the sensory cells of the brain,
- which act on the vaso-motor centre, and this reacts on the small
- arteries of the face, relaxing them and allowing them to become filled
- with blood. Here, again, it seems not improbable that if we were
- repeatedly to concentrate with great earnestness our attention on the
- recollection of our heated faces, the same part of the sensorium which
- gives us the consciousness of actual heat would be in some slight
- degree stimulated, and would in consequence tend to transmit some
- nerve-force to the vaso-motor centres, so as to relax the capillaries
- of the face. Now as men during endless generations have had their
- attention often and earnestly directed to their personal appearance,
- and especially to their faces, any incipient tendency in the facial
- capillaries to be thus affected will have become in the course of time
- greatly strengthened through the principles just referred to, namely,
- nerve-force passing readily along accustomed channels, and inherited
- habit. Thus, as it appears to me, a plausible explanation is afforded
- of the leading phenomena connected with the act of blushing.
- _Recapitulation_.—Men and women, and especially the young, have always
- valued, in a high degree, their personal appearance; and have likewise
- regarded the appearance of others. The face has been the chief object
- of attention, though, when man aboriginally went naked, the whole
- surface of his body would have been attended to. Our self-attention is
- excited almost exclusively by the opinion of others, for no person
- living in absolute solitude would care about his appearance. Every one
- feels blame more acutely than praise. Now, whenever we know, or
- suppose, that others are depreciating our personal appearance, our
- attention is strongly drawn towards ourselves, more especially to our
- faces. The probable effect of this will be, as has just been explained,
- to excite into activity that part of the sensorium, which receives the
- sensory nerves of the face; and this will react through the vaso-motor
- system on the facial capillaries. By frequent reiteration during
- numberless generations, the process will have become so habitual, in
- association with the belief that others are thinking of us, that even a
- suspicion of their depreciation suffices to relax the capillaries,
- without any conscious thought about our faces. With some sensitive
- persons it is enough even to notice their dress to produce the same
- effect. Through the force, also, of association and inheritance our
- capillaries are relaxed, whenever we know, or imagine, that any one is
- blaming, though in silence, our actions, thoughts, or character; and,
- again, when we are highly praised.
- On this hypothesis we can understand how it is that the face blushes
- much more than any other part of the body, though the whole surface is
- somewhat affected, more especially with the races which still go nearly
- naked. It is not at all surprising that the dark-coloured races should
- blush, though no change of colour is visible in their skins. From the
- principle of inheritance it is not surprising that persons born blind
- should blush. We can understand why the young are much more affected
- than the old, and women more than men; and why the opposite sexes
- especially excite each other’s blushes. It becomes obvious why personal
- remarks should be particularly liable to cause blushing, and why the
- most powerful of all the causes is shyness; for shyness relates to the
- presence and opinion of others, and the shy are always more or less
- self-conscious. With respect to real shame from moral delinquencies, we
- can perceive why it is not guilt, but the thought that others think us
- guilty, which raises a blush. A man reflecting on a crime committed in
- solitude, and stung by his conscience, does not blush; yet he will
- blush under the vivid recollection of a detected fault, or of one
- committed in the presence of others, the degree of blushing being
- closely related to the feeling of regard for those who have detected,
- witnessed, or suspected his fault. Breaches of conventional rules of
- conduct, if they are rigidly insisted on by our equals or superiors,
- often cause more intense blushes even than a detected crime, and an act
- which is really criminal, if not blamed by our equals, hardly raises a
- tinge of colour on our cheeks. Modesty from humility, or from an
- indelicacy, excites a vivid blush, as both relate to the judgment or
- fixed customs of others.
- From the intimate sympathy which exists between the capillary
- circulation of the surface of the head and of the brain, whenever there
- is intense blushing, there will be some, and often great, confusion of
- mind. This is frequently accompanied by awkward movements, and
- sometimes by the involuntary twitching of certain muscles.
- As blushing, according to this hypothesis, is an indirect result of
- attention, originally directed to our personal appearance, that is to
- the surface of the body, and more especially to the face, we can
- understand the meaning of the gestures which accompany blushing
- throughout the world. These consist in hiding the face, or turning it
- towards the ground, or to one side. The eyes are generally averted or
- are restless, for to look at the man who causes us to feel shame or
- shyness, immediately brings home in an intolerable manner the
- consciousness that his gaze is directed on us. Through the principle of
- associated habit, the same movements of the face and eyes are
- practised, and can, indeed, hardly be avoided, whenever we know or
- believe that, others are blaming, or too strongly praising, our moral
- conduct.
- CHAPTER XIV. CONCLUDING REMARKS AND SUMMARY.
- The three leading principles which have determined the chief movements
- of expression—Their inheritance—On the part which the will and
- intention have played in the acquirement of various expressions—The
- instinctive recognition of expression—The bearing of our subject on the
- specific unity of the races of man—On the successive acquirement of
- various expressions by the progenitors of man—The importance of
- expression—Conclusion.
- I have now described, to the best of my ability, the chief expressive
- actions in man, and in some few of the lower animals. I have also
- attempted to explain the origin or development of these actions through
- the three principles given in the first chapter. The first of these
- principles is, that movements which are serviceable in gratifying some
- desire, or in relieving some sensation, if often repeated, become so
- habitual that they are performed, whether or not of any service,
- whenever the same desire or sensation is felt, even in a very weak
- degree.
- Our second principle is that of antithesis. The habit of voluntarily
- performing opposite movements under opposite impulses has become firmly
- established in us by the practice of our whole lives. Hence, if certain
- actions have been regularly performed, in accordance with our first
- principle, under a certain frame of mind, there will be a strong and
- involuntary tendency to the performance of directly opposite actions,
- whether or not these are of any use, under the excitement of an
- opposite frame of mind.
- Our third principle is the direct action of the excited nervous system
- on the body, independently of the will, and independently, in large
- part, of habit. Experience shows that nerve-force is generated and set
- free whenever the cerebro-spinal system is excited. The direction which
- this nerve-force follows is necessarily determined by the lines of
- connection between the nerve-cells, with each other and with various
- parts of the body. But the direction is likewise much influenced by
- habit; inasmuch as nerve-force passes readily along accustomed
- channels.
- The frantic and senseless actions of an enraged man may be attributed
- in part to the undirected flow of nerve-force, and in part to the
- effects of habit, for these actions often vaguely represent the act of
- striking. They thus pass into gestures included under our first
- principle; as when an indignant man unconsciously throws himself into a
- fitting attitude for attacking his opponent, though without any
- intention of making an actual attack. We see also the influence of
- habit in all the emotions and sensations which are called exciting; for
- they have assumed this character from having habitually led to
- energetic action; and action affects, in an indirect manner, the
- respiratory and circulatory system; and the latter reacts on the brain.
- Whenever these emotions or sensations are even slightly felt by us,
- though they may not at the time lead to any exertion, our whole system
- is nevertheless disturbed through the force of habit and association.
- Other emotions and sensations are called depressing, because they have
- not habitually led to energetic action, excepting just at first, as in
- the case of extreme pain, fear, and grief, and they have ultimately
- caused complete exhaustion; they are consequently expressed chiefly by
- negative signs and by prostration. Again, there are other emotions,
- such as that of affection, which do not commonly lead to action of any
- kind, and consequently are not exhibited by any strongly marked outward
- signs. Affection indeed, in as far as it is a pleasurable sensation,
- excites the ordinary signs of pleasure.
- On the other hand, many of the effects due to the excitement of the
- nervous system seem to be quite independent of the flow of nerve-force
- along the channels which have been rendered habitual by former
- exertions of the will. Such effects, which often reveal the state of
- mind of the person thus affected, cannot at present be explained; for
- instance, the change of colour in the hair from extreme terror or
- grief,—the cold sweat and the trembling of the muscles from fear,—the
- modified secretions of the intestinal canal,—and the failure of certain
- glands to act.
- Notwithstanding that much remains unintelligible in our present
- subject, so many expressive movements and actions can be explained to a
- certain extent through the above three principles, that we may hope
- hereafter to see all explained by these or by closely analogous
- principles.
- Actions of all kinds, if regularly accompanying any state of the mind,
- are at once recognized as expressive. These may consist of movements of
- any part of the body, as the wagging of a dog’s tail, the shrugging of
- a man’s shoulders, the erection of the hair, the exudation of
- perspiration, the state of the capillary circulation, laboured
- breathing, and the use of the vocal or other sound-producing
- instruments. Even insects express anger, terror, jealousy, and love by
- their stridulation. With man the respiratory organs are of especial
- importance in expression, not only in a direct, but in a still higher
- degree in an indirect manner.
- Few points are more interesting in our present subject than the
- extraordinarily complex chain of events which lead to certain
- expressive movements. Take, for instance, the oblique eyebrows of a man
- suffering from grief or anxiety. When infants scream loudly from hunger
- or pain, the circulation is affected, and the eyes tend to become
- gorged with blood: consequently the muscles surrounding the eyes are
- strongly contracted as a protection: this action, in the course of many
- generations, has become firmly fixed and inherited: but when, with
- advancing years and culture, the habit of screaming is partially
- repressed, the muscles round the eyes still tend to contract, whenever
- even slight distress is felt: of these muscles, the pyramidals of the
- nose are less under the control of the will than are the others and
- their contraction can be checked only by that of the central fasciae of
- the frontal muscle: these latter fasciae draw up the inner ends of the
- eyebrows, and wrinkle the forehead in a peculiar manner, which we
- instantly recognize as the expression of grief or anxiety. Slight
- movements, such as these just described, or the scarcely perceptible
- drawing down of the corners of the mouth, are the last remnants or
- rudiments of strongly marked and intelligible movements. They are as
- full of significance to us in regard to expression, as are ordinary
- rudiments to the naturalist in the classification and genealogy of
- organic beings.
- That the chief expressive actions, exhibited by man and by the lower
- animals, are now innate or inherited,—that is, have not been learnt by
- the individual,—is admitted by every one. So little has learning or
- imitation to do with several of them that they are from the earliest
- days and throughout life quite beyond our control; for instance, the
- relaxation of the arteries of the skin in blushing, and the increased
- action of the heart in anger. We may see children, only two or three
- years old, and even those born blind, blushing from shame; and the
- naked scalp of a very young infant reddens from passion. Infants scream
- from pain directly after birth, and all their features then assume the
- same form as during subsequent years. These facts alone suffice to show
- that many of our most important expressions have not been learnt; but
- it is remarkable that some, which are certainly innate, require
- practice in the individual, before they are performed in a full and
- perfect manner; for instance, weeping and laughing. The inheritance of
- most of our expressive actions explains the fact that those born blind
- display them, as I hear from the Rev. R. H. Blair, equally well with
- those gifted with eyesight. We can thus also understand the fact that
- the young and the old of widely different races, both with man and
- animals, express the same state of mind by the same movements.
- We are so familiar with the fact of young and old animals displaying
- their feelings in the same manner, that we hardly perceive how
- remarkable it is that a young puppy should wag its tail when pleased,
- depress its ears and uncover its canine teeth when pretending to be
- savage, just like an old dog; or that a kitten should arch its little
- back and erect its hair when frightened and angry, like an old cat.
- When, however, we turn to less common gestures in ourselves, which we
- are accustomed to look at as artificial or conventional,—such as
- shrugging the shoulders, as a sign of impotence, or the raising the
- arms with open hands and extended fingers, as a sign of wonder,—we feel
- perhaps too much surprise at finding that they are innate. That these
- and some other gestures are inherited, we may infer from their being
- performed by very young children, by those born blind, and by the most
- widely distinct races of man. We should also bear in mind that new and
- highly peculiar tricks, in association with certain states of the mind,
- are known to have arisen in certain individuals, and to have been
- afterwards transmitted to their offspring, in some cases, for more than
- one generation.
- Certain other gestures, which seem to us so natural that we might
- easily imagine that they were innate, apparently have been learnt like
- the words of a language. This seems to be the case with the joining of
- the uplifted hands, and the turning up of the eyes, in prayer. So it is
- with kissing as a mark of affection; but this is innate, in so far as
- it depends on the pleasure derived from contact with a beloved person.
- The evidence with respect to the inheritance of nodding and shaking the
- head, as signs of affirmation and negation, is doubtful; for they are
- not universal, yet seem too general to have been independently acquired
- by all the individuals of so many races.
- We will now consider how far the will and consciousness have come into
- play in the development of the various movements of expression. As far
- as we can judge, only a few expressive movements, such as those just
- referred to, are learnt by each individual; that is, were consciously
- and voluntarily performed during the early years of life for some
- definite object, or in imitation of others, and then became habitual.
- The far greater number of the movements of expression, and all the more
- important ones, are, as we have seen, innate or inherited; and such
- cannot be said to depend on the will of the individual. Nevertheless,
- all those included under our first principle were at first voluntarily
- performed for a definite object,—namely, to escape some danger, to
- relieve some distress, or to gratify some desire. For instance, there
- can hardly be a doubt that the animals which fight with their teeth,
- have acquired the habit of drawing back their ears closely to their
- heads, when feeling savage, from their progenitors having voluntarily
- acted in this manner in order to protect their ears from being torn by
- their antagonists; for those animals which do not fight with their
- teeth do not thus express a savage state of mind. We may infer as
- highly probable that we ourselves have acquired the habit of
- contracting the muscles round the eyes, whilst crying gently, that is,
- without the utterance of any loud sound, from our progenitors,
- especially during infancy, having experienced, during the act of
- screaming, an uncomfortable sensation in their eyeballs. Again, some
- highly expressive movements result from the endeavour to cheek or
- prevent other expressive movements; thus the obliquity of the eyebrows
- and the drawing down of the corners of the mouth follow from the
- endeavour to prevent a screaming-fit from coming on, or to cheek it
- after it has come on. Here it is obvious that the consciousness and
- will must at first have come into play; not that we are conscious in
- these or in other such cases what muscles are brought into action, any
- more than when we perform the most ordinary voluntary movements.
- With respect to the expressive movements due to the principle of
- antithesis, it is clear that the will has intervened, though in a
- remote and indirect manner. So again with the movements coming under
- our third principle; these, in as far as they are influenced by
- nerve-force readily passing along habitual channels, have been
- determined by former and repeated exertions of the will. The effects
- indirectly due to this latter agency are often combined in a complex
- manner, through the force of habit and association, with those directly
- resulting from the excitement of the cerebro-spinal system. This seems
- to be the case with the increased action of the heart under the
- influence of any strong emotion. When an animal erects its hair,
- assumes a threatening attitude, and utters fierce sounds, in order to
- terrify an enemy, we see a curious combination of movements which were
- originally voluntary with those that are involuntary. It is, however,
- possible that even strictly involuntary actions, such as the erection
- of the hair, may have been affected by the mysterious power of the
- will.
- Some expressive movements may have arisen spontaneously, in association
- with certain states of the mind, like the tricks lately referred to,
- and afterwards been inherited. But I know of no evidence rendering this
- view probable.
- The power of communication between the members of the same tribe by
- means of language has been of paramount importance in the development
- of man; and the force of language is much aided by the expressive
- movements of the face and body. We perceive this at once when we
- converse on an important subject with any person whose face is
- concealed. Nevertheless there are no grounds, as far as I can discover,
- for believing that any muscle has been developed or even modified
- exclusively for the sake of expression. The vocal and other
- sound-producing organs, by which various expressive noises are
- produced, seem to form a partial exception; but I have elsewhere
- attempted to show that these organs were first developed for sexual
- purposes, in order that one sex might call or charm the other. Nor can
- I discover grounds for believing that any inherited movement, which now
- serves as a means of expression, was at first voluntarily and
- consciously performed for this special purpose,—like some of the
- gestures and the finger-language used by the deaf and dumb. On the
- contrary, every true or inherited movement of expression seems to have
- had some natural and independent origin. But when once acquired, such
- movements may be voluntarily and consciously employed as a means of
- communication. Even infants, if carefully attended to, find out at a
- very early age that their screaming brings relief, and they soon
- voluntarily practise it. We may frequently see a person voluntarily
- raising his eyebrows to express surprise, or smiling to express
- pretended satisfaction and acquiescence. A man often wishes to make
- certain gestures conspicuous or demonstrative, and will raise his
- extended arms with widely opened fingers above his head, to show
- astonishment, or lift his shoulders to his ears, to show that he cannot
- or will not do something. The tendency to such movements will be
- strengthened or increased by their being thus voluntarily and
- repeatedly performed; and the effects may be inherited.
- It is perhaps worth consideration whether movements at first used only
- by one or a few individuals to express a certain state of mind may not
- sometimes have spread to others, and ultimately have become universal,
- through the power of conscious and unconscious imitation. That there
- exists in man a strong tendency to imitation, independently of the
- conscious will, is certain. This is exhibited in the most extraordinary
- manner in certain brain diseases, especially at the commencement of
- inflammatory softening of the brain, and has been called the “echo
- sign.” Patients thus affected imitate, without understanding every
- absurd gesture which is made, and every word which is uttered near
- them, even in a foreign language.[1401] In the case of animals, the
- jackal and wolf have learnt under confinement to imitate the barking of
- the dog. How the barking of the dog, which serves to express various
- emotions and desires, and which is so remarkable from having been
- acquired since the animal was domesticated, and from being inherited in
- different degrees by different breeds, was first learnt we do not know;
- but may we not suspect that imitation has had something to do with its
- acquisition, owing to dogs having long lived in strict association with
- so loquacious an animal as man?
- In the course of the foregoing remarks and throughout this volume, I
- have often felt much difficulty about the proper application of the
- terms, will, consciousness, and intention. Actions, which were at first
- voluntary, soon became habitual, and at last hereditary, and may then
- be performed even in opposition to the will. Although they often reveal
- the state of the mind, this result was not at first either intended or
- expected. Even such words as that “certain movements serve as a means
- of expression,” are apt to mislead, as they imply that this was their
- primary purpose or object. This, however, seems rarely or never to have
- been the case; the movements having been at first either of some direct
- use, or the indirect effect of the excited state of the sensorium. An
- infant may scream either intentionally or instinctively to show that it
- wants food; but it has no wish or intention to draw its features into
- the peculiar form which so plainly indicates misery; yet some of the
- most characteristic expressions exhibited by man are derived from the
- act of screaming, as has been explained.
- Although most of our expressive actions are innate or instinctive, as
- is admitted by everyone, it is a different question whether we have any
- instinctive power of recognizing them. This has generally been assumed
- to be the case; but the assumption has been strongly controverted by M.
- Lemoine.[1402] Monkeys soon learn to distinguish, not only the tones of
- voice of their masters, but the expression of their faces, as is
- asserted by a careful observer.[1403] Dogs well know the difference
- between caressing and threatening gestures or tones; and they seem to
- recognize a compassionate tone. But as far as I can make out, after
- repeated trials, they do not understand any movement confined to the
- features, excepting a smile or laugh; and this they appear, at least in
- some cases, to recognize. This limited amount of knowledge has probably
- been gained, both by monkeys and dogs, through their associating harsh
- or kind treatment with our actions; and the knowledge certainly is not
- instinctive. Children, no doubt, would soon learn the movements of
- expression in their elders in the same manner as animals learn those of
- man. Moreover, when a child cries or laughs, he knows in a general
- manner what he is doing and what he feels; so that a very small
- exertion of reason would tell him what crying or laughing meant in
- others. But the question is, do our children acquire their knowledge of
- expression solely by experience through the power of association and
- reason?
- As most of the movements of expression must have been gradually
- acquired, afterwards becoming instinctive, there seems to be some
- degree of _a priori_ probability that their recognition would likewise
- have become instinctive. There is, at least, no greater difficulty in
- believing this than in admitting that, when a female quadruped first
- bears young, she knows the cry of distress of her offspring, or than in
- admitting that many animals instinctively recognize and fear their
- enemies; and of both these statements there can be no reasonable doubt.
- It is however extremely difficult to prove that our children
- instinctively recognize any expression. I attended to this point in my
- first-born infant, who could not have learnt anything by associating
- with other children, and I was convinced that he understood a smile and
- received pleasure from seeing one, answering it by another, at much too
- early an age to have learnt anything by experience. When this child was
- about four months old, I made in his presence many odd noises and
- strange grimaces, and tried to look savage; but the noises, if not too
- loud, as well as the grimaces, were all taken as good jokes; and I
- attributed this at the time to their being preceded or accompanied by
- smiles. When five months old, he seemed to understand a compassionate,
- expression and tone of voice. When a few days over six months old, his
- nurse pretended to cry, and I saw that his face instantly assumed a
- melancholy expression, with the corners of the mouth strongly
- depressed; now this child could rarely have seen any other child
- crying, and never a grown-up person crying, and I should doubt whether
- at so early an age he could have reasoned on the subject. Therefore it
- seems to me that an innate feeling must have told him that the
- pretended crying of his nurse expressed grief; and this through the
- instinct of sympathy excited grief in him.
- M. Lemoine argues that, if man possessed an innate knowledge of
- expression, authors and artists would not have found it so difficult,
- as is notoriously the case, to describe and depict the characteristic
- signs of each particular state of mind. But this does not seem to me a
- valid argument. We may actually behold the expression changing in an
- unmistakable manner in a man or animal, and yet be quite unable, as I
- know from experience, to analyse the nature of the change. In the two
- photographs given by Duchenne of the same old man (Plate III. figs. 5
- and 6), almost every one recognized that the one represented a true,
- and the other a false smile; but I have found it very difficult to
- decide in what the whole amount of difference consists. It has often
- struck me as a curious fact that so many shades of expression are
- instantly recognized without any conscious process of analysis on our
- part. No one, I believe, can clearly describe a sullen or sly
- expression; yet many observers are unanimous that these expressions can
- be recognized in the various races of man. Almost everyone to whom I
- showed Duchenne’s photograph of the young man with oblique eyebrows
- (Plate II. fig. 2) at once declared that it expressed grief or some
- such feeling; yet probably not one of these persons, or one out of a
- thousand persons, could beforehand have told anything precise about the
- obliquity of the eyebrows with their inner ends puckered, or about the
- rectangular furrows on the forehead. So it is with many other
- expressions, of which I have had practical experience in the trouble
- requisite in instructing others what points to observe. If, then, great
- ignorance of details does not prevent our recognizing with certainty
- and promptitude various expressions, I do not see how this ignorance
- can be advanced as an argument that our knowledge, though vague and
- general, is not innate.
- I have endeavoured to show in considerable detail that all the chief
- expressions exhibited by man are the same throughout the world. This
- fact is interesting, as it affords a new argument in favour of the
- several races being descended from a single parent-stock, which must
- have been almost completely human in structure, and to a large extent
- in mind, before the period at which the races diverged from each other.
- No doubt similar structures, adapted for the same purpose, have often
- been independently acquired through variation and natural selection by
- distinct species; but this view will not explain close similarity
- between distinct species in a multitude of unimportant details. Now if
- we bear in mind the numerous points of structure having no relation to
- expression, in which all the races of man closely agree, and then add
- to them the numerous points, some of the highest importance and many of
- the most trifling value, on which the movements of expression directly
- or indirectly depend, it seems to me improbable in the highest degree
- that so much similarity, or rather identity of structure, could have
- been acquired by independent means. Yet this must have been the case if
- the races of man are descended from several aboriginally distinct
- species. It is far more probable that the many points of close
- similarity in the various races are due to inheritance from a single
- parent-form, which had already assumed a human character.
- It is a curious, though perhaps an idle speculation, how early in the
- long line of our progenitors the various expressive movements, now
- exhibited by man, were successively acquired. The following remarks
- will at least serve to recall some of the chief points discussed in
- this volume. We may confidently believe that laughter, as a sign of
- pleasure or enjoyment, was practised by our progenitors long before
- they deserved to be called human; for very many kinds of monkeys, when
- pleased, utter a reiterated sound, clearly analogous to our laughter,
- often accompanied by vibratory movements of their jaws or lips, with
- the corners of the mouth drawn backwards and upwards, by the wrinkling
- of the cheeks, and even by the brightening of the eyes.
- We may likewise infer that fear was expressed from an extremely remote
- period, in almost the same manner as it now is by man; namely, by
- trembling, the erection of the hair, cold perspiration, pallor, widely
- opened eyes, the relaxation of most of the muscles, and by the whole
- body cowering downwards or held motionless.
- Suffering, if great, will from the first have caused screams or groans
- to be uttered, the body to be contorted, and the teeth to be ground
- together. But our progenitors will not have exhibited those highly
- expressive movements of the features which accompany screaming and
- crying until their circulatory and respiratory organs, and the muscles
- surrounding the eyes, had acquired their present structure. The
- shedding of tears appears to have originated through reflex action from
- the spasmodic contraction of the eyelids, together perhaps with the
- eyeballs becoming gorged with blood during the act of screaming.
- Therefore weeping probably came on rather late in the line of our
- descent; and this conclusion agrees with the fact that our nearest
- allies, the anthropomorphous apes, do not weep. But we must here
- exercise some caution, for as certain monkeys, which are not closely
- related to man, weep, this habit might have been developed long ago in
- a sub-branch of the group from which man is derived. Our early
- progenitors, when suffering from grief or anxiety, would not have made
- their eyebrows oblique, or have drawn down the corners of their mouth,
- until they had acquired the habit of endeavouring to restrain their
- screams. The expression, therefore, of grief and anxiety is eminently
- human.
- Rage will have been expressed at a very early period by threatening or
- frantic gestures, by the reddening of the skin, and by glaring eyes,
- but not by frowning. For the habit of frowning seems to have been
- acquired chiefly from the corrugators being the first muscles to
- contract round the eyes, whenever during infancy pain, anger, or
- distress is felt, and there consequently is a near approach to
- screaming; and partly from a frown serving as a shade in difficult and
- intent vision. It seems probable that this shading action would not
- have become habitual until man had assumed a completely upright
- position, for monkeys do not frown when exposed to a glaring light. Our
- early progenitors, when enraged, would probably have exposed their
- teeth more freely than does man, even when giving full vent to his
- rage, as with the insane. We may, also, feel almost certain that they
- would have protruded their lips, when sulky or disappointed, in a
- greater degree than is the case with our own children, or even with the
- children of existing savage races.
- Our early progenitors, when indignant or moderately angry, would not
- have held their heads erect, opened their chests, squared their
- shoulders, and clenched their fists, until they had acquired the
- ordinary carriage and upright attitude of man, and had learnt to fight
- with their fists or clubs. Until this period had arrived the
- antithetical gesture of shrugging the shoulders, as a sign of impotence
- or of patience, would not have been developed. From the same reason
- astonishment would not then have been expressed by raising the arms
- with open hands and extended fingers. Nor, judging from the actions of
- monkeys, would astonishment have been exhibited by a widely opened
- mouth; but the eyes would have been opened and the eyebrows arched.
- Disgust would have been shown at a very early period by movements round
- the mouth, like those of vomiting,—that is, if the view which I have
- suggested respecting the source of the expression is correct, namely,
- that our progenitors had the power, and used it, of voluntarily and
- quickly rejecting any food from their stomachs which they disliked. But
- the more refined manner of showing contempt or disdain, by lowering the
- eyelids, or turning away the eyes and face, as if the despised person
- were not worth looking at, would not probably have been acquired until
- a much later period.
- Of all expressions, blushing seems to be the most strictly human; yet
- it is common to all or nearly all the races of man, whether or not any
- change of colour is visible in their skin. The relaxation of the small
- arteries of the surface, on which blushing depends, seems to have
- primarily resulted from earnest attention directed to the appearance of
- our own persons, especially of our faces, aided by habit, inheritance,
- and the ready flow of nerve-force along accustomed channels; and
- afterwards to have been extended by the power of association to
- self-attention directed to moral conduct. It can hardly be doubted that
- many animals are capable of appreciating beautiful colours and even
- forms, as is shown by the pains which the individuals of one sex take
- in displaying their beauty before those of the opposite sex. But it
- does not seem possible that any animal, until its mental powers had
- been developed to an equal or nearly equal degree with those of man,
- would have closely considered and been sensitive about its own personal
- appearance. Therefore we may conclude that blushing originated at a
- very late period in the long line of our descent.
- From the various facts just alluded to, and given in the course of this
- volume, it follows that, if the structure of our organs of respiration
- and circulation had differed in only a slight degree from the state in
- which they now exist, most of our expressions would have been
- wonderfully different. A very slight change in the course of the
- arteries and veins which run to the head, would probably have prevented
- the blood from accumulating in our eyeballs during violent expiration;
- for this occurs in extremely few quadrupeds. In this case we should not
- have displayed some of our most characteristic expressions. If man had
- breathed water by the aid of external branchiae (though the idea is
- hardly conceivable), instead of air through his mouth and nostrils, his
- features would not have expressed his feelings much more efficiently
- than now do his hands or limbs. Rage and disgust, however, would still
- have been shown by movements about the lips and mouth, and the eyes
- would have become brighter or duller according to the state of the
- circulation. If our ears had remained movable, their movements would
- have been highly expressive, as is the case with all the animals which
- fight with their teeth; and we may infer that our early progenitors
- thus fought, as we still uncover the canine tooth on one side when we
- sneer at or defy any one, and we uncover all our teeth when furiously
- enraged.
- The movements of expression in the face and body, whatever their origin
- may have been, are in themselves of much importance for our welfare.
- They serve as the first means of communication between the mother and
- her infant; she smiles approval, and thus encourages her child on the
- right path, or frowns disapproval. We readily perceive sympathy in
- others by their expression; our sufferings are thus mitigated and our
- pleasures increased; and mutual good feeling is thus strengthened. The
- movements of expression give vividness and energy to our spoken words.
- They reveal the thoughts and intentions of others more truly than do
- words, which may be falsified. Whatever amount of truth the so-called
- science of physiognomy may contain, appears to depend, as Haller long
- ago remarked,[1404] on different persons bringing into frequent use
- different facial muscles, according to their dispositions; the
- development of these muscles being perhaps thus increased, and the
- lines or furrows on the face, due to their habitual contraction, being
- thus rendered deeper and more conspicuous. The free expression by
- outward signs of an emotion intensifies it. On the other hand, the
- repression, as far as this is possible, of all outward signs softens
- our emotions.[1405] He who gives way to violent gestures will increase
- his rage; he who does not control the signs of fear will experience
- fear in a greater degree; and he who remains passive when overwhelmed
- with grief loses his best chance of recovering elasticity of mind.
- These results follow partly from the intimate relation which exists
- between almost all the emotions and their outward manifestations; and
- partly from the direct influence of exertion on the heart, and
- consequently on the brain. Even the simulation of an emotion tends to
- arouse it in our minds. Shakespeare, who from his wonderful knowledge
- of the human mind ought to be an excellent judge, says:—
- Is it not monstrous that this player here,
- But in a fiction, in a dream of passion,
- Could force his soul so to his own conceit,
- That, from her working, all his visage wann’d;
- Tears in his eyes, distraction in ’s aspect,
- A broken voice, and his whole function suiting
- With forms to his conceit? And all for nothing!
- _Hamlet_, act ii. sc. 2.
- We have seen that the study of the theory of expression confirms to a
- certain limited extent the conclusion that man is derived from some
- lower animal form, and supports the belief of the specific or
- sub-specific unity of the several races; but as far as my judgment
- serves, such confirmation was hardly needed. We have also seen that
- expression in itself, or the language of the emotions, as it has
- sometimes been called, is certainly of importance for the welfare of
- mankind. To understand, as far as possible, the source or origin of the
- various expressions which may be hourly seen on the faces of the men
- around us, not to mention our domesticated animals, ought to possess
- much interest for us. From these several causes, we may conclude that
- the philosophy of our subject has well deserved the attention which it
- has already received from several excellent observers, and that it
- deserves still further attention, especially from any able
- physiologist.
- FOOTNOTES:
- 1 (return) [ J. Parsons, in his paper in the Appendix to the
- ‘Philosophical Transactions’ for 1746, p. 41, gives a list of forty-one
- old authors who have written on Expression.]
- 2 (return) [ Conférences sur l’expression des différents Caractères des
- Passions.’ Paris, 4to, 1667. I always quote from the republication of
- the ‘Conférences’ in the edition of Lavater, by Moreau, which appeared
- in 1820, as given in vol. ix. p. 257.]
- 3 (return) [ ‘Discours par Pierre Camper sur le moyen de représenter
- les diverses passions,’ &c. 1792. 1844]
- 4 (return) [ I always quote from the third edition, 1844, which was
- published after the death of Sir C. Bell, and contains his latest
- corrections. The first edition of 1806 is much inferior in merit, and
- does not include some of his more important views.]
- 5 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie et de la Parole,’ par Albert Lemoine,
- 1865, p. 101.]
- 6 (return) [ ‘L’Art de connaître les Hommes,’ &c., par G. Lavater. The
- earliest edition of this work, referred to in the preface to the
- edition of 1820 in ten volumes, as containing the observations of M.
- Moreau, is said to have been published in 1807; and I have no doubt
- that this is correct, because the ‘Notice sur Lavater’ at the
- commencement of volume i. is dated April 13, 1806. In some
- bibliographical works, however, the date of 1805—1809 is given, but it
- seems impossible that 1805 can be correct. Dr. Duchenne remarks
- (‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’-8vo edit. 1862, p. 5, and
- ‘Archives Générales de Médecine,’ Jan. et Fév. 1862) that M. Moreau “_a
- composé pour son ouvrage un article important_,” &c., in the year 1805;
- and I find in volume i. of the edition of 1820 passages bearing the
- dates of December 12, 1805, and another January 5, 1806, besides that
- of April 13, 1806, above referred to. In consequence of some of these
- passages having thus been _composed_ in 1805, Dr. Duchenne assigns to
- M. Moreau the priority over Sir C. Bell, whose work, as we have seen,
- was published in 1806. This is a very unusual manner of determining the
- priority of scientific works; but such questions are of extremely
- little importance in comparison with their relative merits. The
- passages above quoted from M. Moreau and from Le Brun are taken in this
- and all other cases from the edition of 1820 of Lavater, tom. iv. p.
- 228, and tom. ix. p. 279.]
- 7 (return) [ ‘Handbuch der Systematischen Anatomie des Menschen.’ Band
- I. Dritte Abtheilung, 1858.]
- 8 (return) [ ‘The Senses and the Intellect,’ 2nd edit. 1864, pp. 96 and
- 288. The preface to the first edition of this work is dated June, 1855.
- See also the 2nd edition of Mr. Bain’s work on the ‘Emotions and
- Will.’]
- 9 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. p. 121.]
- 10 (return) [ ‘Essays, Scientific, Political, and Speculative,’ Second
- Series, 1863, p. 111. There is a discussion on Laughter in the First
- Series of Essays, which discussion seems to me of very inferior value.]
- 11 (return) [ Since the publication of the essay just referred to, Mr.
- Spencer has written another, on “Morals and Moral Sentiments,” in the
- ‘Fortnightly Review,’ April 1, 1871, p. 426. He has, also, now
- published his final conclusions in vol. ii. of the second edit. of the
- ‘Principles of Psychology,’ 1872, p. 539. I may state, in order that I
- may not be accused of trespassing on Mr. Spencer’s domain, that I
- announced in my ‘Descent of Man,’ that I had then written a part of the
- present volume: my first MS. notes on the subject of expression bear
- the date of the year 1838.]
- 12 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. pp. 98, 121, 131.]
- 13 (return) [ Professor Owen expressly states (Proc. Zoolog. Soc. 1830,
- p. 28) that this is the case with respect to the Orang, and specifies
- all the more important muscles which are well known to serve with man
- for the expression of his feelings. See, also, a description of several
- of the facial muscles in the Chimpanzee, by Prof. Macalister, in
- ‘Annals and Magazine of Natural History,’ vol. vii. May, 1871, p. 342.]
- 14 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ pp. 121, 138.]
- 15 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ pp. 12, 73.]
- 16 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ 8vo edit. p. 31.]
- 17 (return) [ ‘Elements of Physiology,’ English translation, vol. ii.
- p. 934.]
- 18 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. p. 198.]
- 19 (return) [ See remarks to this effect in Lessing’s ‘Lacooon,’
- translated by W. Ross, 1836, p. 19.]
- 20 (return) [ Mr. Partridge in Todd’s ‘Cyclopædia of Anatomy and
- Physiology,’ vol. ii. p. 227.]
- 21 (return) [ ‘La Physionomie,’ par G. Lavater, tom. iv. 1820, p. 274.
- On the number of the facial muscles, see vol. iv. pp. 209-211.]
- 22 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ 1867, s. 91.]
- 101 (return) [ Mr. Herbert Spencer (‘Essays,’ Second Series, 1863, p.
- 138) has drawn a clear distinction between emotions and sensations, the
- latter being “generated in our corporeal framework.” He classes as
- Feelings both emotions and-sensations.]
- 102 (return) [ Müller, ‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat. vol.
- ii. p. 939. See also Mr. H. Spencer’s interesting speculations on the
- same subject, and on the genesis of nerves, in his ‘Principles of
- Biology,’ vol. ii. p. 346; and in his ‘Principles of Psychology,’ 2nd
- edit. pp. 511-557.]
- 103 (return) [ A remark to much the same effect was made long ago by
- Hippocrates and by the illustrious Harvey; for both assert that a young
- animal forgets in the course of a few days the art of sucking, and
- cannot without some difficulty again acquire it. I give these
- assertions on the authority of Dr. Darwin, ‘Zoonomia,’ 1794, vol. i. p.
- 140.]
- 104 (return) [ See for my authorities, and for various analogous facts,
- ‘The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication,’ 1868, vol.
- ii. p. 304.]
- 105 (return) [ ‘The Senses and the Intellect,’ 2nd edit. 1864, p. 332.
- Prof. Huxley remarks (‘Elementary Lessons in Physiology,’ 5th edit.
- 1872, p. 306), “It may be laid down as a rule, that, if any two mental
- states be called up together, or in succession, with due frequency and
- vividness, the subsequent production of the one of them will suffice to
- call up the other, and that whether we desire it or not.”]
- 106 (return) [ Gratiolet (‘De la Physionomie,’ p. 324), in his
- discussion on this subject, gives many analogous instances. See p. 42,
- on the opening and shutting of the eyes. Engel is quoted (p. 323) on
- the changed paces of a man, as his thoughts change.]
- 107 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ 1862, p. 17.]
- 108 (return) [ ‘The Variation of Animals and Plants under
- Domestication,’ vol. ii. p. 6. The inheritance of habitual gestures is
- so important for us, that I gladly avail myself of Mr. F. Galton’s
- permission to give in his own words the following remarkable case:—“The
- following account of a habit occurring in individuals of three
- consecutive generations {footnote continues:} is of peculiar interest,
- because it occurs only during sound sleep, and therefore cannot be due
- to imitation, but must be altogether natural. The particulars are
- perfectly trustworthy, for I have enquired fully into them, and speak
- from abundant and independent evidence. A gentleman of considerable
- position was found by his wife to have the curious trick, when he lay
- fast asleep on his back in bed, of raising his right arm slowly in
- front of his face, up to his forehead, and then dropping it with a
- jerk, so that the wrist fell heavily on the bridge of his nose. The
- trick did not occur every night, but occasionally, and was independent
- of any ascertained cause. Sometimes it was repeated incessantly for an
- hour or more. The gentleman’s nose was prominent, and its bridge often
- became sore from the blows which it received. At one time an awkward
- sore was produced, that was long in healing, on account of the
- recurrence, night after night, of the blows which first caused it. His
- wife had to remove the button from the wrist of his night-gown as it
- made severe scratches, and some means were attempted of tying his arm.
- “Many years after his death, his son married a lady who had never heard
- of the family incident. She, however, observed precisely the same
- peculiarity in her husband; but his nose, from not being particularly
- prominent, has never as yet suffered from the blows. The trick does not
- occur when he is half-asleep, as, for example, when dozing in his
- arm-chair, but the moment he is fast asleep it is apt to begin. It is,
- as with his father, intermittent; sometimes ceasing for many nights,
- and sometimes almost incessant during a part of every night. It is
- performed, as it was by his father, with his right hand.
- “One of his children, a girl, has inherited the same trick. She
- performs it, likewise, with the right hand, but in a slightly modified
- form; for, after raising the arm, she does not allow the wrist to drop
- upon the bridge of the nose, but the palm of the half-closed hand falls
- over and down the nose, striking it rather rapidly. It is also very
- intermittent with this child, not occurring for periods of some months,
- but sometimes occurring almost incessantly.”]
- 109 (return) [ Prof. Huxley remarks (‘Elementary Physiology,’ 5th edit.
- p. 305) that reflex actions proper to the spinal cord are _natural_;
- but, by the help of the brain, that is through habit, an infinity of
- _artificial_ reflex actions may be acquired. Virchow admits (‘Sammlung
- wissenschaft. Vorträge,’ &c., “Ueber das Rückenmark,” 1871, ss. 24, 31)
- that some reflex actions can hardly be distinguished from instincts;
- and, of the latter, it may be added, some cannot be distinguished from
- inherited habits.]
- 110 (return) [ Dr. Maudsley, ‘Body and Mind,’ 1870, p. 8.]
- 111 (return) [ See the very interesting discussion on the whole subject
- by Claude Bernard, ‘Tissus Vivants,’ 1866, p. 353-356.]
- 112 (return) [ ‘Chapters on Mental Physiology,’ 1858, p. 85.]
- 113 (return) [ Müller remarks (‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. tr. vol.
- ii. p. 1311) on starting being always accompanied by the closure of the
- eyelids.]
- 114 (return) [ Dr. Maudsley remarks (‘Body and Mind,’ p. 10) that
- “reflex movements which commonly effect a useful end may, under the
- changed circumstances of disease, do great mischief, becoming even the
- occasion of violent suffering and of a most painful death.”]
- 115 (return) [ See Mr. F. H. Salvin’s account of a tame jackal in ‘Land
- and Water,’ October, 1869.]
- 116 (return) [ “Dr. Darwin, ‘Zoonomia,’ 1794, vol. i. p. 160. I find
- that the fact of cats protruding their feet when pleased is also
- noticed (p. 151) in this work.]
- 117 (return) [ Carpenter, ‘Principles of Comparative Physiology,’ 1854,
- p. 690, and Müller’s ‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat. vol. ii.
- p. 936.]
- 118 (return) [ Mowbray on ‘Poultry,’ 6th edit. 1830, p. 54.]
- 119 (return) [ See the account given by this excellent observer in
- ‘Wild Sports of the Highlands,’ 1846, p. 142.]
- 120 (return) [ ‘Philosophical Translations,’ 1823, p. 182.]
- 201 (return) [ ‘Naturgeschichte der Säugethiere von Paraguay,’ 1830, s.
- 55.]
- 202 (return) [ Mr. Tylor gives an account of the Cistercian
- gesture-language in his ‘Early History of Mankind’ (2nd edit. 1870, p.
- 40), and makes some remarks on the principle of opposition in
- gestures.]
- 203 (return) [ See on this subject Dr. W. R. Scott’s interesting work,
- ‘The Deaf and Dumb,’ 2nd edit. 1870, p. 12. He says, “This contracting
- of natural gestures into much shorter gestures than the natural
- expression requires, is very common amongst the deaf and dumb. This
- contracted gesture is frequently so shortened as nearly to lose all
- semblance of the natural one, but to the deaf and dumb who use it, it
- still has the force of the original expression.”]
- 301 (return) [ See the interesting cases collected by M. G. Pouchet in
- the ‘Revue des Deux Mondes,’ January 1, 1872, p. 79. An instance was
- also brought some years ago before the British Association at Belfast.]
- 302 (return) [ Müller remarks (‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat.
- vol. ii. p. 934) that when the feelings are very intense, “all the
- spinal nerves become affected to the extent of imperfect paralysis, or
- the excitement of trembling of the whole body.”]
- 303 (return) [ ‘Leçons sur les Prop. des Tissus Vivants,’ 1866, pp.
- 457-466.]
- 304 (return) [ Mr. Bartlett, “Notes on the Birth of a Hippopotamus,”
- Proc. Zoolog. Soc. 1871, p. 255.]
- 305 (return) [ See, on this subject, Claude Bernard, ‘Tissus Vivants,’
- 1866, pp. 316, 337, 358. Virchow expresses himself to almost exactly
- the same effect in his essay “Ueber das Rückenmark” (Sammlung
- wissenschaft. Vorträge, 1871, s. 28).]
- 306 (return) [ Müller (‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat. vol.
- ii. p. 932) in speaking of the nerves, says, “any sudden change of
- condition of whatever kind sets the nervous principle into action.” See
- Virchow and Bernard on the same subject in passages in the two works
- referred to in my last foot-note.]
- 307 (return) [ H. Spencer, ‘Essays, Scientific, Political,’ &c., Second
- Series, 1863, pp. 109, 111.]
- 308 (return) [ Sir H. Holland, in speaking (‘Medical Notes and
- Reflexions,’ 1839, p. 328) of that curious state of body called the
- _fidgets_, remarks that it seems due to “an accumulation of some cause
- of irritation which requires muscular action for its relief.”]
- 309 (return) [ I am much indebted to Mr. A. H. Garrod for having
- informed me of M. Lorain’s work on the pulse, in which a sphygmogram of
- a woman in a rage is given; and this shows much difference in the rate
- and other characters from that of the same woman in her ordinary
- state.]
- 310 (return) [ How powerfully intense joy excites the brain, and how
- the brain reacts on the body, is well shown in the rare cases of
- Psychical Intoxication. Dr. J. Crichton Browne (‘Medical Mirror,’ 1865)
- records the case of a young man of strongly nervous temperament, who,
- on hearing by a telegram that a fortune had been bequeathed him, first
- became pale, then exhilarated, and soon in the highest spirits, but
- flushed and very restless. He then took a walk with a friend for the
- sake of tranquillising himself, but returned staggering in his gait,
- uproariously laughing, yet irritable in temper, incessantly talking,
- and singing loudly in the public streets. It was positively ascertained
- that he had not touched any spirituous liquor, though every one thought
- that he was intoxicated. Vomiting after a time came on, and the
- half-digested contents of his stomach were examined, but no odour of
- alcohol could be detected. He then slept heavily, and on awaking was
- well, except that he suffered from headache, nausea, and prostration of
- strength.]
- 311 (return) [ Dr. Darwin, ‘Zoonomia,’ 1794, vol. i. p. 148.]
- 312 (return) [ Mrs. Oliphant, in her novel of ‘Miss Majoribanks,’ p.
- 362. All this reacts on the brain, and prostration soon follows with
- collapsed muscles and dulled eyes. As associated habit no longer
- prompts the sufferer to action, he is urged by his friends to voluntary
- exertion, and not to give way to silent, motionless grief. Exertion
- stimulates the heart, and this reacts on the brain, and aids the mind
- to bear its heavy load.]
- 401 (return) [ See the evidence on this head in my ‘Variation of
- Animals and Plants under Domestication,’ vol. i. p. 27. On the cooing
- of pigeons, vol. i. pp. 154, 155.]
- 402 (return) [ ‘Essays, Scientific, Political, and Speculative,’ 1858.
- ‘The Origin and Function of Music,’ p. 359.]
- 403 (return) [ ‘The Descent of Man,’ 1870, vol. ii. p. 332. The words
- quoted are from Professor Owen. It has lately been shown that some
- quadrupeds much lower in the scale than monkeys, namely Rodents, are
- able to produce correct musical tones: see the account of a singing
- Hesperomys, by the Rev. S. Lockwood, in the ‘American Naturalist,’ vol.
- v. December, 1871, p. 761.]
- 404 (return) [ Mr. Tylor (‘Primitive Culture,’ 1871, vol. i. p. 166),
- in his discussion on this subject, alludes to the whining of the dog.]
- 405 (return) [ ‘Naturgeschichte der Säugethiere von Paraguay,’ 1830, s.
- 46.]
- 406 (return) [ Quoted by Gratiolet, ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 115.]
- 407 (return) [ ‘Théorie Physiologique de la Musique,’ Paris, 1868, P.
- 146. Helmholtz has also fully discussed in this profound work the
- relation of the form of the cavity of the mouth to the production of
- vowel-sounds.]
- 408 (return) [ I have given some details on this subject in my ‘Descent
- of Man,’ vol. i. pp. 352, 384.]
- 409 (return) [ As quoted in Huxley’s ‘Evidence as to Man’s Place in
- Nature,’ 1863, p. 52.]
- 410 (return) [ Illust. Thierleben, 1864, B. i. s. 130.]
- 411 (return) [ The Hon. J. Caton, Ottawa Acad. of Nat. Sciences, May,
- 1868, pp. 36, 40. For the _Capra, Ægagrus_, ‘Land and Water,’ 1867, p.
- 37.]
- 412 (return) [ ‘Land and Water,’ July 20, 1867, p. 659.]
- 413 (return) [ _Phaeton rubricauda_: ‘Ibis,’ vol. iii. 1861, p. 180.]
- 414 (return) [ On the _Strix flammea_, Audubon, ‘Ornithological
- Biography,’ 1864, vol. ii. p. 407. I have observed other cases in the
- Zoological Gardens.]
- 415 (return) [ _Melopsittacus undulatus_. See an account of its habits
- by Gould, ‘Handbook of Birds of Australia,’ 1865, vol. ii. p. 82.]
- 416 (return) [ See, for instance, the account which I have given
- (‘Descent of Man,’ vol. ii. p. 32) of an Anolis and Draco.]
- 417 (return) [ These muscles are described in his well-known works. I
- am greatly indebted to this distinguished observer for having given me
- in a letter information on this same subject.]
- 418 (return) [ ‘Lehrbuch der Histologie des Menschen,’ 1857, s. 82. I
- owe to Prof. W. Turner’s kindness an extract from this work.]
- 419 (return) [ ‘Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science,’ 1853, vol.
- i. p. 262.]
- 420 (return) [ ‘Lehrbuch der Histologie,’ 1857, s. 82.]
- 421 (return) [ ‘Dictionary of English Etymology,’ p. 403.]
- 422 (return) [ See the account of the habits of this animal by Dr.
- Cooper, as quoted in ‘Nature,’ April 27, 1871, p. 512.]
- 423 (return) [ Dr. Günther, ‘Reptiles of British India,’ p. 262.]
- 424 (return) [ Mr. J. Mansel Weale, ‘Nature,’ April 27, 1871, p. 508.]
- 425 (return) [ ‘Journal of Researches during the Voyage of the
- “Beagle,”’ 1845, p. 96. I have compared the rattling thus produced with
- that of the Rattle-snake.]
- 426 (return) [ See the account by Dr. Anderson, Proc. Zool. Soc. 1871,
- p. 196.]
- 427 (return) [ The ‘American Naturalist,’ Jan. 1872, p. 32. I regret
- that I cannot follow Prof. Shaler in believing that the rattle has been
- developed, by the aid of natural selection, for the sake of producing
- sounds which deceive and attract birds, so that they may serve as prey
- to the snake. I do not, however, wish to doubt that the sounds may
- occasionally subserve this end. But the conclusion at which I have
- arrived, viz. that the rattling serves as a warning to would-be
- devourers, appears to me much more probable, as it connects together
- various classes of facts. If this snake had acquired its rattle and the
- habit of rattling, for the sake of attracting prey, it does not seem
- probable that it would have invariably used its instrument when angered
- or disturbed. Prof. Shaler takes nearly the same view as I do of the
- manner of development of the rattle; and I have always held this
- opinion since observing the Trigonocephalus in South America.]
- 428 (return) [ From the accounts lately collected, and given in the
- ‘Journal of the Linnean Society,’ by Airs. Barber, on the habits of the
- snakes of South Africa; and from the accounts published by several
- writers, for instance by Lawson, of the rattle-snake in North
- America,—it does not seem improbable that the terrific appearance of
- snakes and the sounds produced by them, may likewise serve in procuring
- prey, by paralysing, or as it is sometimes called fascinating, the
- smaller animals.]
- 429 (return) [ See the account by Dr. R. Brown, in Proc. Zool. Soc.
- 1871, p. 39. He says that as soon as a pig sees a snake it rushes upon
- it; and a snake makes off immediately on the appearance of a pig.]
- 430 (return) [ Dr. Günther remarks (‘Reptiles of British India,’ p.
- 340) on the destruction of cobras by the ichneumon or herpestes, and
- whilst the cobras are young by the jungle-fowl. It is well known that
- the peacock also eagerly kills snakes.]
- 431 (return) [ Prof. Cope enumerates a number of kinds in his ‘Method
- of Creation of Organic Types,’ read before the American Phil. Soc.,
- December 15th, 1871, p. 20. Prof. Cope takes the same view as I do of
- the use of the gestures and sounds made by snakes. I briefly alluded to
- this subject in the last edition of my ‘Origin of Species.’ Since the
- passages in the text above have been printed, I have been pleased to
- find that Mr. Henderson (‘The American Naturalist,’ May, 1872, p. 260)
- also takes a similar view of the use of the rattle, namely “in
- preventing an attack from being made.”]
- 432 (return) [ Mr. des Vœux, in Proc. Zool. Soc. 1871, p. 3.]
- 433 (return) [ ‘The Sportsman and Naturalist in Canada,’ 1866, p. 53.
- p. 53.{sic}]
- 434 (return) [ ‘The Nile Tributaries of Abyssinia,’ 1867, p. 443.]
- 501 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ 1844, p. 190.]
- 502 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, pp. 187, 218.]
- 503 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ 1844, p. 140.]
- 504 (return) [ Many particulars are given by Gueldenstädt in his
- account of the jackal in Nov. Comm. Acad. Sc. Imp. Petrop. 1775, tom.
- xx. p. 449. See also another excellent account of the manners of this
- animal and of its play, in ‘Land and Water,’ October, 1869. Lieut.
- Annesley, R. A., has also communicated to me some particulars with
- respect to the jackal. I have made many inquiries about wolves and
- jackals in the Zoological Gardens, and have observed them for myself.]
- 505 (return) [ ‘Land and Water,’ November 6, 1869.]
- 506 (return) [ Azara, ‘Quadrupèdes du Paraquay,’ 1801, tom. 1. p. 136.]
- 507 (return) [ ‘Land and Water,’ 1867, p. 657. See also Azara on the
- Puma, in the work above quoted.]
- 508 (return) [ Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. p. 123.
- See also p. 126, on horses not breathing through their mouths, with
- reference to their distended nostrils.]
- 509 (return) [ ‘Land and Water,’ 1869, p. 152.]
- 510 (return) [ ‘Natural History of Mammalia,’ 1841, vol. 1. pp. 383,
- 410.]
- 511 (return) [ Rengger (‘Sagetheire von Paraquay’, 1830, s. 46) kept
- these monkeys in confinement for seven years in their native country of
- Paraguay.]
- 512 (return) [ Rengger, ibid. s. 46. Humboldt, ‘Personal Narrative,
- Eng. translat. vol. iv. p. 527.]
- 513 (return) [ Nat. Hist. of Mammalia, 1841, p. 351.]
- 514 (return) [ Brehm, ‘Thierleben,’ B. i. s. 84. On baboons striking
- the ground, s. 61.]
- 515 (return) [ Brehm remarks (‘Thierleben,’ s. 68) that the eyebrows of
- the _Inuus ecaudatus_ are frequently moved up and down when the animal
- is angered.]
- 516 (return) [ G. Bennett, ‘Wanderings in New South Wales,’ &c. vol.
- ii. 1834, p. 153. FIG. 18.-Chimpanzee disappointed and sulky. Drawn
- from life by Mr. Wood.]
- 517 (return) [ W. L. Martin, Nat. Hist. of Mamm. Animals, 1841, p.
- 405.]
- 518 (return) [ Prof. Owen on the Orang, Proc. Zool. Soc. 1830, p. 28.
- On the Chimpanzee, see Prof. Macalister, in Annals and Mag. of Nat.
- Hist. vol. vii. 1871, p. 342, who states that the _corrugator
- supercilii_ is inseparable from the _orbicularis palpebrarum_.]
- 519 (return) [ Boston Journal of Nat. Hist. 1845—-47, vol. v. p. 423.
- On the Chimpanzee, ibid. 1843-44, vol. iv. p. 365.]
- 520 (return) [ See on this subject, ‘Descent of Man,’ vol. i. p. 20.]
- 521 (return) [ ‘Descent of Man,’ vol, i. p, 43.]
- 522 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. 1844, pp. 138, 121.]
- 601 (return) [ The best photographs in my collection are by Mr.
- Rejlander, of Victoria Street, London, and by Herr Kindermann, of
- Hamburg. Figs. 1, 3, 4, and 6 are by the former; and figs. 2 and 5, by
- the latter gentleman. Fig. 6 is given to show moderate crying in an
- older child.]
- 602 (return) [ Henle (‘Handbuch d. Syst. Anat. 1858, B. i. s. 139)
- agrees with Duchenne that this is the effect of the contraction of the
- _pyramidalis nasi_.]
- 603 (return) [ These consist of the _levator labii superioris alaeque
- nasi_, the _levator labii proprius_, the _malaris_, and the
- _zygomaticus minor_, or little zygomatic. This latter muscle runs
- parallel to and above the great zygomatic, and is attached to the outer
- part of the upper lip. It is represented in fig. 2 (I. p. 24), but not
- in figs. 1 and 3. Dr. Duchenne first showed (‘Mécanisme de la
- Physionomie Humaine,’ Album, 1862, p. 39) the importance of the
- contraction of this muscle in the shape assumed by the features in
- crying. Henle considers the above-named muscles (excepting the
- _malaris_) as subdivisions of the _quadratus labii superioris_.]
- 604 (return) [ Although Dr. Duchenne has so carefully studied the
- contraction of the different muscles during the act of crying, and the
- furrows on the face thus produced, there seems to be something
- incomplete in his account; but what this is I cannot say. He has given
- a figure (Album, fig. 48) in which one half of the face is made, by
- galvanizing the proper muscles, to smile; whilst the other half is
- similarly made to begin crying. Almost all those (viz. nineteen out of
- twenty-one persons) to whom I showed the smiling half of the face
- instantly recognized the expression; but, with respect to the other
- half, only six persons out of twenty-one recognized it,—that is, if we
- accept such terms as “grief,” “misery,” “annoyance,” as
- correct;—whereas, fifteen persons were ludicrously mistaken; some of
- them saying the face expressed “fun,” “satisfaction,” “cunning,”
- “disgust,” &c. We may infer from this that there is something wrong in
- the expression. Some of the fifteen persons may, however, have been
- partly misled by not expecting to see an old man crying, and by tears
- not being secreted. With respect to another figure by Dr. Duchenne
- (fig. 49), in which the muscles of half the face are galvanized in
- order to represent a man beginning to cry, with the eyebrow on the same
- side rendered oblique, which is characteristic of misery, the
- expression was recognized by a greater proportional number of persons.
- Out of twenty-three persons, fourteen answered correctly, “sorrow,”
- “distress,” “grief,” “just going to cry,” “endurance of pain,” &c. On
- the other hand, nine persons either could form no opinion or were
- entirely wrong, answering, “cunning leer,” “jocund,” “looking at an
- intense light,” “looking at a distant object,” &c.]
- 605 (return) [ Mrs. Gaskell, ‘Mary Barton,’ new edit. p. 84.]
- 606 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ 1867, s. 102. Duchenne,
- Mécanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, p. 34.]
- 607 (return) [ Dr. Duchenne makes this remark, ibid. p. 39.]
- 608 (return) [ ‘The Origin of Civilization,’ 1870, p. 355.]
- 609 (return) [ See, for instance, Mr. Marshall’s account of an idiot in
- Philosoph. Transact. 1864, p. 526. With respect to cretins, see Dr.
- Piderit, ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ 1867, s. 61.]
- 610 (return) [ ‘New Zealand and its Inhabitants,’ 1855, p. 175.]
- 611 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 126.]
- 612 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ 1844, p. 106. See also his
- paper in the ‘Philosophical Transactions,’ 1822, p. 284, ibid. 1823,
- pp. 166 and 289. Also ‘The Nervous System of the Human Body,’ 3rd edit.
- 1836, p. 175.]
- 613 (return) [ See Dr. Brinton’s account of the act of vomiting, in
- Todd’s Cyclop. of Anatomy and Physiology, 1859, vol. v. Supplement, p.
- 318.]
- 614 (return) [ I am greatly indebted to Mr. Bowman for having
- introduced me to Prof. Donders, and for his aid in persuading this
- great physiologist to undertake the investigation of the present
- subject. I am likewise much indebted to Mr. Bowman for having given me,
- with the utmost kindness, information on many points.]
- 615 (return) [ This memoir first appeared in the ‘Nederlandsch Archief
- voor Genees en Natuurkunde,’ Deel 5, 1870. It has been translated by
- Dr. W. D. Moore, under the title of “On the Action of the Eyelids in
- determination of Blood from expiratory effort,” in ‘Archives of
- Medicine,’ edited by Dr. L. S. Beale, 1870, vol. v. p. 20.]
- 616 (return) [ Prof. Donders remarks (ibid. p. 28), that, “After injury
- to the eye, after operations, and in some forms of internal
- inflammation, we attach great value to the uniform support of the
- closed eyelids, and we increase this in many instances by the
- application of a bandage. In both cases we carefully endeavour to avoid
- great expiratory pressure, the disadvantage of which is well known.”
- Mr. Bowman informs me that in the excessive photophobia, accompanying
- what is called scrofulous ophthalmia in children, when the light is so
- very painful that during weeks or months it is constantly excluded by
- the most forcible closure of the lids, he has often been struck on
- opening the lids by the paleness of the eye,—not an unnatural paleness,
- but an absence of the redness that might have been expected when the
- surface is somewhat inflamed, as is then usually the case; and this
- paleness he is inclined to attribute to the forcible closure of the
- eyelids.]
- 617 (return) [ Donders, ibid. p. 36.]
- 618 (return) [ Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood (Dict. of English Etymology,
- 1859, vol. i. p. 410) says, “the verb to weep comes from Anglo-Saxon
- _wop_, the primary meaning of which is simply outcry.”]
- 619 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 217.]
- 620 (return) [ ‘Ceylon,’ 3rd edit. 1859, vol. ii. pp. 364, 376. I
- applied to Mr. Thwaites, in Ceylon, for further information with
- respect to the weeping of the elephant; and in consequence received a
- letter from the Rev. Mr Glenie, who, with others, kindly observed for
- me a herd of recently captured elephants. These, when irritated,
- screamed violently; but it is remarkable that they never when thus
- screaming contracted the muscles round the eyes. Nor did they shed
- tears; and the native hunters asserted that they had never observed
- elephants weeping. Nevertheless, it appears to me impossible to doubt
- Sir E. Tennent’s distinct details about their weeping, supported as
- they are by the positive assertion of the keeper in the Zoological
- Gardens. It is certain that the two elephants in the Gardens, when they
- began to trumpet loudly, invariably contracted their orbicular muscles.
- I can reconcile these conflicting statements only by supposing that the
- recently captured elephants in Ceylon, from being enraged or
- frightened, desired to observe their persecutors, and consequently did
- not contract their orbicular muscles, so that their vision might not be
- impeded. Those seen weeping by Sir E. Tennent were prostrate, and had
- given up the contest in despair. The elephants which trumpeted in the
- Zoological Gardens at the word of command, were, of course, neither
- alarmed nor enraged.]
- 621 (return) [ Bergeon, as quoted in the ‘Journal of Anatomy and
- Physiology,’ Nov. 1871, p. 235.]
- 622 (return) [ See, for instance, a case given by Sir Charles Bell,
- ‘Philosophical Transactions,’ 1823, p. 177.]
- 623 (return) [ See, on these several points, Prof. Donders ‘On the
- Anomalies of Accommodation and Refraction of the Eye,’ 1864, p. 573.]
- 624 (return) [ Quoted by Sir J. Lubbock, ‘Prehistoric Times,’ 1865, p.
- 458.]
- 701 (return) [ The above descriptive remarks are taken in part from my
- own observations, but chiefly from Gratiolet (‘De la Physionomie,’ pp.
- 53, 337; on Sighing, 232), who has well treated this whole subject.
- See, also, Huschke, ‘Mimices et Physiognomices, Fragmentum
- Physiologi-cum,’ 1821, p. 21. On the dulness of the eyes, Dr. Piderit,
- ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ 1867, s. 65.]
- 702 (return) [ On the action of grief on the organs of respiration, see
- more especially Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ 3rd edit. 1844,
- p. 151.]
- 703 (return) [ In the foregoing remarks on the manner in which the
- eyebrows are made oblique, I have followed what seems to be the
- universal opinion of all the anatomists, whose works I have consulted
- on the action of the above-named muscles, or with whom I have
- conversed. Hence throughout this work I shall take a similar view of
- the action of the corrugator supercilii, orbicularis, pyramidalis nasi,
- and frontalis muscles. Dr. Duchenne, however, believes, and every
- conclusion at which he arrives deserves serious consideration, that it
- is the corrugator, called by him the sourcilier, which raises the inner
- corner of the eyebrows and is antagonistic to the upper and inner part
- of the orbicular muscle, as well as to the pyramidalis nasi (see
- Mécanisme de la Phys. Humaine, 1862, folio, art. v., text and figures
- 19 to 29: octavo edit. 1862, p. 43 text). He admits, however, that the
- corrugator draws together the eyebrows, causing vertical furrows above
- the base of the nose, or a frown. He further believes that towards the
- outer two-thirds of the eyebrow the corrugator acts in conjunction with
- the upper orbicular muscle; both here standing in antagonism to the
- frontal muscle. I am unable to understand, judging from Henle’s
- drawings (woodcut, fig. 3), how the corrugator can act in the manner
- described by Duchenne. See, also, on this subject, Prof. Donders’
- remarks in the ‘Archives of Medicine,’ 1870, vol. v. p. 34. Mr. J.
- Wood, who is so well known for his careful study of the muscles of the
- human frame, informs me that he believes the account which I have given
- of the action of the corrugator to be correct. But this is not a point
- of any importance with respect to the expression which is caused by the
- obliquity of the eyebrows, nor of much importance to the theory of its
- origin.]
- 704 (return) [ I am greatly indebted to Dr. Duchenne for permission to
- have these two photographs (figs. 1 and 2) reproduced by the heliotype
- process from his work in folio. Many of the foregoing remarks on the
- furrowing of the skin, when the eyebrows are rendered oblique, are
- taken from his excellent discussion on this subject.]
- 705 (return) [ Mécanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, p. 15.]
- 706 (return) [ Henle, Handbuch der Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s.
- 148, figs. 68 and 69.]
- 707 (return) [ See the account of the action of this muscle by Dr.
- Duchenne, ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine, Album (1862), viii. p.
- 34.]
- 801 (return) [ Herbert Spencer, ‘Essays Scientific,’ &c., 1858, p.
- 360.]
- 802 (return) [ F. Lieber on the vocal sounds of L. Bridgman,
- ‘Smithsonian Contributions,’ 1851, vol. ii. p. 6.]
- 803 (return) [ See, also, Mr. Marshall, in Phil. Transact. 1864, p.
- 526.]
- 804 (return) [ Mr. Bain (‘The Emotions and the Will,’ 1865, p. 247) has
- a long and interesting discussion on the Ludicrous. The quotation above
- given about the laughter of the gods is taken from this work. See,
- also, Mandeville, ‘The Fable of the Bees,’ vol. ii. p. 168.]
- 805 (return) [ ‘The Physiology of Laughter,’ Essays, Second Series,
- 1863, p. 114.]
- 806 (return) [ J. Lister in ‘Quarterly Journal of Microscopical
- Science,’ 1853, vol. 1. p. 266.]
- 807 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ p. 186.]
- 808 (return) [ Sir C. Bell (Anat. of Expression, p. 147) makes some
- remarks on the movement of the diaphragm during laughter.]
- 809 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ Album, Légende
- vi.]
- 810 (return) [ Handbuch der System. Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s.
- 144. See my woodcut (H. fig. 2).]
- 811 (return) [ See, also, remarks to the same effect by Dr. J. Crichton
- Browne in ‘Journal of Mental Science,’ April, 1871, p. 149.]
- 812 (return) [ C. Vogt, ‘Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’ 1867, p. 21.]
- 813 (return) [ Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 133.]
- 814 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ 1867, s. 63-67.]
- 815 (return) [ Sir T. Reynolds remarks (‘Discourses,’ xii. p. 100), “it
- is curious to observe, and it is certainly true, that the extremes of
- contrary passions are, with very little variation, expressed by the
- same action.” He gives as an instance the frantic joy of a Bacchante
- and the grief of a Mary Magdalen.]
- 816 (return) [ Dr. Piderit has come to the same conclusion, ibid. s.
- 99.]
- 817 (return) [ ‘La Physionomie,’ par G. Lavater, edit. of 1820, vol.
- iv. p. 224. See, also, Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 172,
- for the quotation given below.]
- 818 (return) [ A ‘Dictionary of English Etymology,’ 2nd edit. 1872,
- Introduction, p. xliv.]
- 819 (return) [ Crantz, quoted by Tylor, ‘Primitive Culture,’ 1871, Vol.
- i. P. 169.]
- 820 (return) [ F. Lieber, ‘Smithsonian Contributions,’ 1851, vol. ii.
- p. 7.]
- 821 (return) [ Mr. Bain remarks (‘Mental and Moral Science,’ 1868, p.
- 239), “Tenderness is a pleasurable emotion, variously stimulated, whose
- effort is to draw human beings into mutual embrace.”]
- 822 (return) [ Sir J. Lubbock, ‘Prehistoric Times,’ 2nd edit. 1869, p.
- 552, gives full authorities for these statements. The quotation from
- Steele is taken from this work.]
- 823 (return) [ See a full acount,{sic} with references, by E. B. Tylor,
- ‘Researches into the Early History of Mankind,’ 2nd edit. 1870, p. 51.]
- 824 (return) [ ‘The Descent of Man,’ vol. ii. p. 336.]
- 825 (return) [ Dr. Mandsley has a discussion to this effect in his
- ‘Body and Mind,’ 1870, p. 85.]
- 826 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 103, and ‘Philosophical
- Transactions,’ 1823, p. 182.]
- 827 (return) [ ‘The Origin of Language,’ 1866, p. 146. Mr. Tylor
- (‘Early History of Mankind,’ 2nd edit. 1870, p. 48) gives a more
- complex origin to the position of the hands during prayer.]
- 901 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ pp. 137, 139. It is not
- surprising that the corrugators should have become much more developed
- in man than in the anthropoid apes; for they are brought into incessant
- action by him under various circumstances, and will have been
- strengthened and modified by the inherited effects of use. We have seen
- how important a part they play, together with the orbiculares, in
- protecting the eyes from being too much gorged with blood during
- violent expiratory movements. When the eyes are closed as quickly and
- as forcibly as possible, to save them from being injured by a blow, the
- corrugators contract. With savages or other men whose heads are
- uncovered, the eyebrows are continually lowered and contracted to serve
- as a shade against a too strong light; and this is effected partly by
- the corrugators. This movement would have been more especially
- serviceable to man, as soon as his early progenitors held their heads
- erect. Lastly, Prof. Donders believes (‘Archives of Medicine,’ ed. by
- L. Beale, 1870, vol. v. p. 34), that the corrugators are brought into
- action in causing the eyeball to advance in accommodation for proximity
- in vision.]
- 902 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ Album, Légende
- iii.]
- 903 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ s. 46.]
- 904 (return) [ ‘History of the Abipones,’ Eng. translat. vol. ii. p.
- 59, as quoted by Lubbock, ‘Origin of Civilisation,’ 1870, p. 355.]
- 905 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ pp. 15, 144, 146. Mr. Herbert
- Spencer accounts for frowning exclusively by the habit of contracting
- the brows as a shade to the eyes in a bright light: see ‘Principles of
- Physiology,’ 2nd edit. 1872, p. 546.]
- 906 (return) [ Gratiolet remarks (De la Phys. p. 35), “Quand
- l’attention est fixee sur quelque image interieure, l’oeil regarde dons
- le vide et s’associe automatiquement a la contemplation de l’esprit.”
- But this view hardly deserves to be called an explanation.]
- 907 (return) [ ‘Miles Gloriosus,’ act ii. sc. 2.]
- 908 (return) [ The original photograph by Herr Kindermann is much more
- expressive than this copy, as it shows the frown on the brow more
- plainly.]
- 909 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine,’ Album, Légende
- iv. figs. 16-18.]
- 910 (return) [ Hensleigh Wedgwood on ‘The Origin of Language,’ 1866, p.
- 78.]
- 911 (return) [ Müller, as quoted by Huxley, ‘Man’s Place in Nature,’
- 1863, p. 38.]
- 912 (return) [ I have given several instances in my ‘Descent of Man,’
- vol. i. chap. iv.]
- 913 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression.’ p. 190.]
- 914 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ pp. 118-121.]
- 915 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ s. 79.]
- 1001 (return) [ See some remarks to this effect by Mr. Bain, ‘The
- Emotions and the Will,’ 2nd edit. 1865, p. 127.]
- 1002 (return) [ Rengger, Naturgesch. der Säugethiere von Paraguay,
- 1830, s. 3.]
- 1003 (return) [ Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 96. On the
- other hand, Dr. Burgess (‘Physiology of Blushing,’ 1839, p. 31) speaks
- of the reddening of a cicatrix in a negress as of the nature of a
- blush.]
- 1004 (return) [ Moreau and Gratiolet have discussed the colour of the
- face under the influence of intense passion: see the edit. of 1820 of
- Lavater, vol. iv. pp. 282 and 300; and Gratiolet, ‘De la Physionomie,’
- p. 345.]
- 1005 (return) [ Sir C. Bell ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ pp. 91, 107, has
- fully discussed this subject. Moreau remarks (in the edit. of 1820 of
- ‘La Physionomie, par G. Lavater,’ vol. iv. p. 237), and quotes Portal
- in confirmation, that asthmatic patients acquire permanently expanded
- nostrils, owing to the habitual contraction of the elevatory muscles of
- the wings of the nose. The explanation by Dr. Piderit (‘Mimik und
- Physiognomik,’ s. 82) of the distension of the nostrils, namely, to
- allow free breathing whilst the mouth is closed and the teeth clenched,
- does not appear to be nearly so correct as that by Sir C. Bell, who
- attributes it to the sympathy (_i. e_. habitual co-action) of all the
- respiratory muscles. The nostrils of an angry man may be seen to become
- dilated, although his mouth is open.]
- 1006 (return) [ Mr. Wedgwood, ‘On the Origin of Language,’ 1866, p. 76.
- He also observes that the sound of hard breathing “is represented by
- the syllables _puff, huff, whiff_, whence a _huff_ is a fit of
- ill-temper.”]
- 1007 (return) [ Sir C. Bell ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 95) has some
- excellent remarks on the expression of rage.]
- 1008 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 346.]
- 1009 (return) [ Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 177. Gratiolet
- (De la Phys. p. 369) says, ‘les dents se découvrent, et imitent
- symboliquement l’action de déchirer et de mordre.’I If, instead of
- using the vague term _symboliquement_, Gratiolet had said that the
- action was a remnant of a habit acquired during primeval times when our
- semi-human progenitors fought together with their teeth, like gorillas
- and orangs at the present day, he would have been more intelligible.
- Dr. Piderit (‘Mimik,’ &c., s. 82) also speaks of the retraction of the
- upper lip during rage. In an engraving of one of Hogarth’s wonderful
- pictures, passion is represented in the plainest manner by the open
- glaring eyes, frowning forehead, and exposed grinning teeth.]
- 1010 (return) [ ‘Oliver Twist,’ vol. iii. p. 245.]
- 1011 (return) [ ‘The Spectator,’ July 11, 1868, p. 810.]
- 1012 (return) [ ‘Body and Mind,’ 1870, pp. 51-53.]
- 1013 (return) [ Le Brun, in his well-known ‘Conference sur
- l’Expression’ (‘La Physionomie, par Lavater,’ edit. of 1820, vol. lx.
- p. 268), remarks that anger is expressed by the clenching of the fists.
- See, to the same effect, Huschke, ‘Mimices et Physiognomices,
- Fragmentum Physiologicum,’ 1824, p. 20. Also Sir C. Bell, ‘Anatomy of
- Expression,’ p. 219.]
- 1014 (return) [ Transact. Philosoph. Soc., Appendix, 1746, p. 65.]
- 1015 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 136. Sir C. Bell calls (p.
- 131) the muscles which uncover the canines the snarling muscles.]
- 1016 (return) [ Hensleigh Wedgwood, ‘Dictionary of English Etymology,’
- 1865, vol. iii. pp. 240, 243.]
- 1017 (return) [ ‘The Descent of Man,’ 1871, vol. L p. 126.]
- 1101 (return) [ ‘De In Physionomie et la Parole,’ 1865, p. 89.]
- 1102 (return) [ ‘Physionomie Humaine,’ Album, Légende viii. p. 35.
- Gratiolet also speaks (De la Phys. 1865, p. 52) of the turning away of
- the eyes and body.]
- 1103 (return) [ Dr. W. Ogle, in an interesting paper on the Sense of
- Smell (‘Medico-Chirurgical Transactions,’ vol. liii. p. 268), shows
- that when we wish to smell carefully, instead of taking one deep nasal
- inspiration, we draw in the air by a succession of rapid short sniffs.
- If “the nostrils be watched during this process, it will be seen that,
- so far from dilating, they actually contract at each sniff. The
- contraction does not include the whole anterior opening, but only the
- posterior portion.” He then explains the cause of this movement. When,
- on the other hand, we wish to exclude any odour, the contraction, I
- presume, affects only the anterior part of the nostrils.]
- 1104 (return) [ ‘Mimik und Physiognomik,’ ss. 84, 93. Gratiolet (ibid.
- p. 155) takes nearly the same view with Dr. Piderit respecting the
- expression of contempt and disgust.]
- 1105 (return) [ Scorn implies a strong form of contempt; and one of the
- roots of the word ‘scorn’ means, according to Mr. Wedgwood (Dict. of
- English Etymology, vol. iii. p. 125), ordure or dirt. A person who is
- scorned is treated like dirt.]
- 1106 (return) [ ‘Early History of Mankind,’ 2nd edit. 1870, p. 45.]
- 1107 (return) [ See, to this effect, Mr. Hensleigh Wedgwood’s
- Introduction to the ‘Dictionary of English Etymology,’ 2nd edit. 1872,
- p. xxxvii.]
- 1108 (return) [ Duchenne believes that in the eversion of the lower
- lip, the corners are drawn downwards by the _depressores anguli oris_.
- Henle (Handbuch d. Anat. des Menschen, 1858, B. i. s. 151) concludes
- that this is effected by the _musculus quadratus menti_.]
- 1109 (return) [ As quoted by Tylor, ‘Primitive Culture,’ 1871, vol. i.
- p. 169.]
- 1110 (return) [ Both these quotations are given by Mr. H. Wedgwood, ‘On
- the Origin of Language,’ 1866, p. 75.]
- 1111 (return) [ This is stated to be the case by Mr. Tylor (Early Hist.
- of Mankind, 2nd edit. 1870, p. 52); and he adds, “it is not clear why
- this should be so.”]
- 1112 (return) [ ‘Principles of Psychology,’ 2nd edit. 1872, p. 552.]
- 1113 (return) [ Gratiolet (De la Phys. p. 351) makes this remark, and
- has some good observations on the expression of pride. See Sir C. Bell
- (‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 111) on the action of the _musculus
- superbus_.]
- 1114 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 166.]
- 1115 (return) [ ‘Journey through Texas,’ p. 352.]
- 1116 (return) [ Mrs. Oliphant, ‘The Brownlows,’ vol. ii. p. 206.]
- 1117 (return) [ ‘Essai sur le Langage,’ 2nd edit. 1846. I am much
- indebted to Miss Wedgwood for having given me this information, with an
- extract from the work.]
- 1118 (return) [ ‘On the Origin of Language,’ 1866, p. 91.]
- 1119 (return) [ ‘On the Vocal Sounds of L. Bridgman;’ Smithsonian
- Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 11.]
- 1120 (return) [ ‘Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’ 1867, p. 27.]
- 1121 (return) [ Quoted by Tylor, ‘Early History of Mankind,’ 2nd edit.
- 1870, p. 38.]
- 1122 (return) [ Mr. J. B. Jukes, ‘Letters and Extracts,’ &c. 1871, p.
- 248.]
- 1123 (return) [ F. Lieber, ‘On the Vocal Sounds,’ &c. p. 11. Tylor,
- ibid. p. 53.]
- 1124 (return) [ Dr. King, Edinburgh Phil. Journal, 1845, p. 313.]
- 1125 (return) [ Tylor, ‘Early History of Mankind,’ 2nd edit. 1870, p.
- 53.]
- 1126 (return) [ Lubbock, ‘The Origin of Civilization,’ 1870, p. 277.
- Tylor, ibid. p. 38. Lieber (ibid. p. 11) remarks on the negative of the
- Italians.]
- 1201 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie,’ Album, 1862, p. 42.]
- 1202 (return) [ ‘The Polyglot News Letter,’ Melbourne, Dec. 1858, p.
- 2.]
- 1203 (return) [ ‘The Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 106.]
- 1204 (return) [ Mécanisme de la Physionomie,’ Album, p. 6.]
- 1205 (return) [ See, for instance, Dr. Piderit (‘Mimik und
- Physiognomik,’ s. 88), who has a good discussion on the expression of
- surprise.]
- 1206 (return) [ Dr. Murie has also given me information leading to the
- same conclusion, derived in part from comparative anatomy.]
- 1207 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 234.]
- 1208 (return) [ See, on this subject, Gratiolet, ibid. p. 254.]
- 1209 (return) [ Lieber, ‘On the Vocal Sounds of Laura Bridgman,’
- Smithsonian Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 7.]
- 1210 (return) [ ‘Wenderholme,’ vol. ii. p. 91.]
- 1211 (return) [ Lieber, ‘On the Vocal Sounds,’ &c., ibid. p. 7.]
- 1212 (return) [ Huschke, ‘Mimices et Physiognomices,’ 1821, p. 18.
- Gratiolet (De la Phys. p. 255) gives a figure of a man in this
- attitude, which, however, seems to me expressive of fear combined with
- astonishment. Le Brun also refers (Lavater, vol. ix. p. 299) to the
- hands of an astonished man being opened.]
- 1213 (return) [ Huschke, ibid. p. 18.]
- 1214 (return) [ ‘North American Indians,’ 3rd edit. 1842, vol. i. p.
- 105.]
- 1215 (return) [ H. Wedgwood, Dict. of English Etymology, vol. ii. 1862,
- p. 35. See, also, Gratiolet (‘De la Physionomie,’ p. 135) on the
- sources of such words as ‘terror, horror, rigidus, frigidus,’ &c.]
- 1216 (return) [ Mr. Bain (‘The Emotions and the Will,’ 1865, p. 54)
- explains in the following manner the origin of the custom “of
- subjecting criminals in India to the ordeal of the morsel of rice. The
- accused is made to take a mouthful of rice, and after a little time to
- throw it out. If the morsel is quite dry, the party is believed to be
- guilty,—his own evil conscience operating to paralyse the salivating
- organs.”]
- 1217 (return) [ Sir C. Bell, Transactions of Royal Phil. Soc. 1822, p.
- 308. ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 88 and pp. 164-469.]
- 1218 (return) [ See Moreau on the rolling of the eyes, in the edit. of
- 1820 of Lavater, tome iv. p. 263. Also, Gratiolet, De la Phys. p. 17.]
- 1219 (return) [ ‘Observations on Italy,’ 1825, p. 48, as quoted in ‘The
- Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 168.]
- 1220 (return) [ Quoted by Dr. Maudsley, ‘Body and Mind,’ 1870, p. 41.]
- 1221 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 168.]
- 1222 (return) [ Mécanisme de la Phys. Humaine, Album, Légende xi.]
- 1223 (return) [ Ducheinne takes, in fact, this view (ibid. p. 45), as
- he attributes the contraction of the platysma to the shivering of fear
- (_frisson de la peur_); but he elsewhere compares the action with that
- which causes the hair of frightened quadrupeds to stand erect; and this
- can hardly be considered as quite correct.]
- 1224 (return) [ ‘De la Physionomie,’ pp. 51, 256, 346.]
- 1225 (return) [ As quoted in White’s ‘Gradation in Man,’ p. 57.]
- 1226 (return) [ ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 169.]
- 1227 (return) [ ‘Mécanisme de la Physionomie,’ Album, pl. 65, pp. 44,
- 45.]
- 1228 (return) [ See remarks to this effect by Mr. Wedgwood, in the
- Introduction to his ‘Dictionary of English Etymology,’ 2nd edit. 1872,
- p. xxxvii. He shows by intermediate forms that the sounds here referred
- to have probably given rise to many words, such as _ugly, huge_, &c.]
- 1301 (return) [ ‘The Physiology or Mechanism of Blushing,’ 1839, p.
- 156. I shall have occasion often to quote this work in the present
- chapter.]
- 1302 (return) [ Dr. Burgess, ibid. p. 56. At p. 33 he also remarks on
- women blushing more freely than men, as stated below.]
- 1303 (return) [ Quoted by Vogt, ‘Mémoire sur les Microcéphales,’ 1867,
- p. 20. Dr. Burgess (ibid. p. 56) doubts whether idiots ever blush.]
- 1304 (return) [ Lieber ‘On the Vocal Sounds,’ &c.; Smithsonian
- Contributions, 1851, vol. ii. p. 6.]
- 1305 (return) [ Ibid. p. 182.]
- 1306 (return) [ Moreau, in edit. of 1820 of Lavater, vol. iv. p. 303.]
- 1307 (return) [ Burgess. ibid. p. 38, on paleness after blushing, p.
- 177.]
- 1308 (return) [ See Lavater, edit. of 1820, vol. iv. p. 303.]
- 1309 (return) [ Burgess, ibid. pp. 114, 122. Moreau in Lavater, ibid.
- vol. iv. p. 293.]
- 1310 (return) [ ‘Letters from Egypt,’ 1865, p. 66. Lady Gordon is
- mistaken when she says Malays and Mulattoes never blush.]
- 1311 (return) [ Capt. Osborn (‘Quedah,’ p. 199), in speaking of a
- Malay, whom he reproached for cruelty, says he was glad to see that the
- man blushed.]
- 1312 (return) [ J. R. Forster, ‘Observations during a Voyage round the
- World,’ 4to, 1778, p. 229. Waitz gives (‘Introduction to Anthropology,’
- Eng. translat. 1863, vol. i. p. 135) references for other islands in
- the Pacific. See, also, Dampier ‘On the Blushing of the Tunquinese’
- (vol. ii. p. 40); but I have not consulted this work. Waitz quotes
- Bergmann, that the Kalmucks do not blush, but this may be doubted after
- what we have seen with respect to the Chinese. He also quotes Roth, who
- denies that the Abyssinians are capable of blushing. Unfortunately,
- Capt. Speedy, who lived so long with the Abyssinians, has not answered
- my inquiry on this head. Lastly, I must add that the Rajah Brooke has
- never observed the least sign of a blush with the Dyaks of Borneo; on
- the contrary under circumstances which would excite a blush in us, they
- assert “that they feel the blood drawn from their faces.”]
- 1313 (return) [ Transact. of the Ethnological Soc. 1870, vol. ii. p.
- 16.]
- 1314 (return) [ Humboldt, ‘Personal Narrative,’ Eng. translat. vol.
- iii. p. 229.]
- 1315 (return) [ Quoted by Prichard, Phys. Hist. of Mankind, 4th edit
- 1851, vol. i. p. 271.]
- 1316 (return) [ See, on this head, Burgess, ibid. p. 32. Also Waitz,
- ‘Introduction to Anthropology,’ Eng. edit. vol. i. p. 139. Moreau gives
- a detailed account (‘Lavater,’ 1820, tom. iv. p. 302) of the blushing
- of a Madagascar negress-slave when forced by her brutal master to
- exhibit her naked bosom.]
- 1317 (return) [ Quoted by Prichard, Phys. Hist. of Mankind, 4th edit.
- 1851, vol. i. p. 225.]
- 1318 (return) [ Burgess, ibid. p. 31. On mulattoes blushing, see p. 33.
- I have received similar accounts with respect to, mulattoes.]
- 1319 (return) [ Barrington also says that the Australians of New South
- Wales blush, as quoted by Waitz, ibid. p. 135.]
- 1320 (return) [ Mr. Wedgwood says (Dict. of English Etymology, vol.
- iii. 1865, p. 155) that the word shame “may well originate in the idea
- of shade or concealment, and may be illustrated by the Low German
- _scheme_, shade or shadow.” Gratiolet (De la Phys. pp. 357-362) has a
- good discussion on the gestures accompanying shame; but some of his
- remarks seem to me rather fanciful. See, also, Burgess (ibid. pp. 69,
- 134) on the same subject.]
- 1321 (return) [ Burgess, ibid. pp. 181, 182. Boerhaave also noticed (as
- quoted by Gratiolet, ibid. p. 361) the tendency to the secretion of
- tears during intense blushing. Mr. Bulmer, as we have seen, speaks of
- the “watery eyes” of the children of the Australian aborigines when
- ashamed.]
- 1322 (return) [ See also Dr. J. Crichton Browne’s Memoir on this
- subject in the ‘West Riding Lunatic Asylum Medical Report,’ 1871, pp.
- 95-98.]
- 1323 (return) [ In a discussion on so-called animal magnetism in ‘Table
- Talk,’ vol. i.]
- 1324 (return) [ Ibid. p. 40.]
- 1325 (return) [ Mr. Bain (‘The Emotions and the Will,’ 1865, p. 65)
- remarks on “the shyness of manners which is induced between the
- sexes.... from the influence of mutual regard, by the apprehension on
- either side of not standing well with the other.”]
- 1326 (return) [ See, for evidence on this subject, ‘The Descent of
- Man,’ &c., vol. ii. pp. 71, 341.]
- 1327 (return) [ H. Wedgwood, Dict. English Etymology, vol. iii. 1865,
- p. 184. So with the Latin word _verecundus_.]
- 1328 (return) [ Mr. Bain (‘The Emotions and the Will,’ p. 64) has
- discussed the “abashed” feelings experienced on these occasions, as
- well as the _stage-fright_ of actors unused to the stage. Mr. Bain
- apparently attributes these feelings to simple apprehension or dread.]
- 1329 (return) [ ‘Essays on Practical Education,’ by Maria and R. L.
- Edgeworth, new edit. vol. ii. 1822, p. 38. Dr. Burgess (ibid. p. 187)
- insists strongly to the same effect.]
- 1330 (return) [ ‘Essays on Practical Education,’ by Maria and R. L.
- Edgeworth, new edit. vol. ii. 1822, p. 50.]
- 1331 (return) [ Bell, ‘Anatomy of Expression,’ p. 95. Burgess, as
- quoted below, ibid. p. 49. Gratiolet, De la Phys. p. 94.]
- 1332 (return) [ On the authority of Lady Mary Wortley Montague; see
- Burgess, ibid. p. 43.]
- 1333 (return) [ In England, Sir H. Holland was, I believe, the first to
- consider the influence of mental attention on various parts of the
- body, in his ‘Medical Notes and Reflections,’ 1839 p. 64. This essay,
- much enlarged, was reprinted by Sir H. Holland in his ‘Chapters on
- Mental Physiology,’ 1858, p. 79, from which work I always quote. At
- nearly the same time, as well as subsequently, Prof. Laycock discussed
- the same subject: see ‘Edinburgh Medical and Surgical Journal,’ 1839,
- July, pp. 17-22. Also his ‘Treatise on the Nervous Diseases of Women,’
- 1840, p. 110; and ‘Mind and Brain,’ vol. ii. 1860, p. 327. Dr.
- Carpenter’s views on mesmerism have a nearly similar bearing. The great
- physiologist Müller treated (‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat.
- vol. ii. pp. 937, 1085) of the influence of the attention on the
- senses. Sir J. Paget discusses the influence of the mind on the
- nutrition of parts, in his ‘Lectures on Surgical Pathology,’ 1853, vol.
- i. p. 39: 1 quote from the 3rd edit. revised by Prof. Turner, 1870, p.
- 28. See, also, Gratiolet, De la Phys. pp. 283-287.]
- 1334 (return) [ De la Phys. p. 283.]
- 1340 (return) [ Dr. Maudsley has given (‘The Physiology and Pathology
- of Mind,’ 2nd edit. 1868, p. 105), on good authority, some curious
- statements with respect to the improvement of the sense of touch by
- practice and attention. It is remarkable that when this sense has thus
- been rendered more acute at any point of the body, for instance, in a
- finger, it is likewise improved at the corresponding point on the
- opposite side of the body.]
- 1341 (return) [ The Lancet,’ 1838, pp. 39-40, as quoted by Prof.
- Laycock, ‘Nervous Diseases of Women,’ 1840, p. 110.]
- 1342 (return) [ ‘Chapters on Mental Physiology,’ 1858, pp. 91-93.]
- 1343 (return) [ ‘Lectures on Surgical Pathology,’ 3rd edit. revised by
- Prof. Turner, 1870, pp. 28, 31.]
- 1344 (return) [ ‘Elements of Physiology,’ Eng. translat. vol. ii. p.
- 938.]
- 1345 (return) [ Prof. Laycock has discussed this point in a very
- interesting manner. See his ‘Nervous Diseases of Women,’ 1840, p. 110.]
- 1346 (return) [ See, also, Mr. Michael Foster, on the action of the
- vaso-motor system, in his interesting Lecture before the royal
- Institution, as translated in the ‘Revue des Cours Scientifiques,’
- Sept. 25, 1869, p. 683.]
- 1401 (return) [ See the interesting facts given by Dr. Bateman on
- ‘Aphasia,’ 1870, p. 110.]
- 1402 (return) [ ‘La Physionomie et la Parole,’ 1865, pp. 103, 118.]
- 1403 (return) [ Rengger, ‘Naturgeschichte der Säugethiere von
- Paraguay,’ 1830, s. 55.]
- 1404 (return) [ Quoted by Moreau, in his edition of Lavater, 1820, tom.
- iv. p. 211.]
- 1405 (return) [ Gratiolet (‘De la Physionomie,’ 1865, p. 66) insists on
- the truth of this conclusion.]
- End of the Project Gutenberg EBook of The Expression of Emotion in Man and
- Animals, by Charles Darwin
- *** END OF THIS PROJECT GUTENBERG EBOOK EMOTION IN MAN AND ANIMALS ***
- ***** This file should be named 1227-0.txt or 1227-0.zip *****
- This and all associated files of various formats will be found in:
- http://www.gutenberg.org/1/2/2/1227/
- Produced by Charles Keller and David Widger
- Updated editions will replace the previous one--the old editions will
- be renamed.
- Creating the works from print editions not protected by U.S. copyright
- law means that no one owns a United States copyright in these works,
- so the Foundation (and you!) can copy and distribute it in the United
- States without permission and without paying copyright
- royalties. Special rules, set forth in the General Terms of Use part
- of this license, apply to copying and distributing Project
- Gutenberg-tm electronic works to protect the PROJECT GUTENBERG-tm
- concept and trademark. Project Gutenberg is a registered trademark,
- and may not be used if you charge for the eBooks, unless you receive
- specific permission. If you do not charge anything for copies of this
- eBook, complying with the rules is very easy. You may use this eBook
- for nearly any purpose such as creation of derivative works, reports,
- performances and research. They may be modified and printed and given
- away--you may do practically ANYTHING in the United States with eBooks
- not protected by U.S. copyright law. Redistribution is subject to the
- trademark license, especially commercial redistribution.
- START: FULL LICENSE
- THE FULL PROJECT GUTENBERG LICENSE
- PLEASE READ THIS BEFORE YOU DISTRIBUTE OR USE THIS WORK
- To protect the Project Gutenberg-tm mission of promoting the free
- distribution of electronic works, by using or distributing this work
- (or any other work associated in any way with the phrase "Project
- Gutenberg"), you agree to comply with all the terms of the Full
- Project Gutenberg-tm License available with this file or online at
- www.gutenberg.org/license.
- Section 1. General Terms of Use and Redistributing Project
- Gutenberg-tm electronic works
- 1.A. By reading or using any part of this Project Gutenberg-tm
- electronic work, you indicate that you have read, understand, agree to
- and accept all the terms of this license and intellectual property
- (trademark/copyright) agreement. If you do not agree to abide by all
- the terms of this agreement, you must cease using and return or
- destroy all copies of Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works in your
- possession. If you paid a fee for obtaining a copy of or access to a
- Project Gutenberg-tm electronic work and you do not agree to be bound
- by the terms of this agreement, you may obtain a refund from the
- person or entity to whom you paid the fee as set forth in paragraph
- 1.E.8.
- 1.B. "Project Gutenberg" is a registered trademark. It may only be
- used on or associated in any way with an electronic work by people who
- agree to be bound by the terms of this agreement. There are a few
- things that you can do with most Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works
- even without complying with the full terms of this agreement. See
- paragraph 1.C below. There are a lot of things you can do with Project
- Gutenberg-tm electronic works if you follow the terms of this
- agreement and help preserve free future access to Project Gutenberg-tm
- electronic works. See paragraph 1.E below.
- 1.C. The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation ("the
- Foundation" or PGLAF), owns a compilation copyright in the collection
- of Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works. Nearly all the individual
- works in the collection are in the public domain in the United
- States. If an individual work is unprotected by copyright law in the
- United States and you are located in the United States, we do not
- claim a right to prevent you from copying, distributing, performing,
- displaying or creating derivative works based on the work as long as
- all references to Project Gutenberg are removed. Of course, we hope
- that you will support the Project Gutenberg-tm mission of promoting
- free access to electronic works by freely sharing Project Gutenberg-tm
- works in compliance with the terms of this agreement for keeping the
- Project Gutenberg-tm name associated with the work. You can easily
- comply with the terms of this agreement by keeping this work in the
- same format with its attached full Project Gutenberg-tm License when
- you share it without charge with others.
- 1.D. The copyright laws of the place where you are located also govern
- what you can do with this work. Copyright laws in most countries are
- in a constant state of change. If you are outside the United States,
- check the laws of your country in addition to the terms of this
- agreement before downloading, copying, displaying, performing,
- distributing or creating derivative works based on this work or any
- other Project Gutenberg-tm work. The Foundation makes no
- representations concerning the copyright status of any work in any
- country outside the United States.
- 1.E. Unless you have removed all references to Project Gutenberg:
- 1.E.1. The following sentence, with active links to, or other
- immediate access to, the full Project Gutenberg-tm License must appear
- prominently whenever any copy of a Project Gutenberg-tm work (any work
- on which the phrase "Project Gutenberg" appears, or with which the
- phrase "Project Gutenberg" is associated) is accessed, displayed,
- performed, viewed, copied or distributed:
- This eBook is for the use of anyone anywhere in the United States and
- most other parts of the world at no cost and with almost no
- restrictions whatsoever. You may copy it, give it away or re-use it
- under the terms of the Project Gutenberg License included with this
- eBook or online at www.gutenberg.org. If you are not located in the
- United States, you'll have to check the laws of the country where you
- are located before using this ebook.
- 1.E.2. If an individual Project Gutenberg-tm electronic work is
- derived from texts not protected by U.S. copyright law (does not
- contain a notice indicating that it is posted with permission of the
- copyright holder), the work can be copied and distributed to anyone in
- the United States without paying any fees or charges. If you are
- redistributing or providing access to a work with the phrase "Project
- Gutenberg" associated with or appearing on the work, you must comply
- either with the requirements of paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 or
- obtain permission for the use of the work and the Project Gutenberg-tm
- trademark as set forth in paragraphs 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.
- 1.E.3. If an individual Project Gutenberg-tm electronic work is posted
- with the permission of the copyright holder, your use and distribution
- must comply with both paragraphs 1.E.1 through 1.E.7 and any
- additional terms imposed by the copyright holder. Additional terms
- will be linked to the Project Gutenberg-tm License for all works
- posted with the permission of the copyright holder found at the
- beginning of this work.
- 1.E.4. Do not unlink or detach or remove the full Project Gutenberg-tm
- License terms from this work, or any files containing a part of this
- work or any other work associated with Project Gutenberg-tm.
- 1.E.5. Do not copy, display, perform, distribute or redistribute this
- electronic work, or any part of this electronic work, without
- prominently displaying the sentence set forth in paragraph 1.E.1 with
- active links or immediate access to the full terms of the Project
- Gutenberg-tm License.
- 1.E.6. You may convert to and distribute this work in any binary,
- compressed, marked up, nonproprietary or proprietary form, including
- any word processing or hypertext form. However, if you provide access
- to or distribute copies of a Project Gutenberg-tm work in a format
- other than "Plain Vanilla ASCII" or other format used in the official
- version posted on the official Project Gutenberg-tm web site
- (www.gutenberg.org), you must, at no additional cost, fee or expense
- to the user, provide a copy, a means of exporting a copy, or a means
- of obtaining a copy upon request, of the work in its original "Plain
- Vanilla ASCII" or other form. Any alternate format must include the
- full Project Gutenberg-tm License as specified in paragraph 1.E.1.
- 1.E.7. Do not charge a fee for access to, viewing, displaying,
- performing, copying or distributing any Project Gutenberg-tm works
- unless you comply with paragraph 1.E.8 or 1.E.9.
- 1.E.8. You may charge a reasonable fee for copies of or providing
- access to or distributing Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works
- provided that
- * You pay a royalty fee of 20% of the gross profits you derive from
- the use of Project Gutenberg-tm works calculated using the method
- you already use to calculate your applicable taxes. The fee is owed
- to the owner of the Project Gutenberg-tm trademark, but he has
- agreed to donate royalties under this paragraph to the Project
- Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation. Royalty payments must be paid
- within 60 days following each date on which you prepare (or are
- legally required to prepare) your periodic tax returns. Royalty
- payments should be clearly marked as such and sent to the Project
- Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation at the address specified in
- Section 4, "Information about donations to the Project Gutenberg
- Literary Archive Foundation."
- * You provide a full refund of any money paid by a user who notifies
- you in writing (or by e-mail) within 30 days of receipt that s/he
- does not agree to the terms of the full Project Gutenberg-tm
- License. You must require such a user to return or destroy all
- copies of the works possessed in a physical medium and discontinue
- all use of and all access to other copies of Project Gutenberg-tm
- works.
- * You provide, in accordance with paragraph 1.F.3, a full refund of
- any money paid for a work or a replacement copy, if a defect in the
- electronic work is discovered and reported to you within 90 days of
- receipt of the work.
- * You comply with all other terms of this agreement for free
- distribution of Project Gutenberg-tm works.
- 1.E.9. If you wish to charge a fee or distribute a Project
- Gutenberg-tm electronic work or group of works on different terms than
- are set forth in this agreement, you must obtain permission in writing
- from both the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation and The
- Project Gutenberg Trademark LLC, the owner of the Project Gutenberg-tm
- trademark. Contact the Foundation as set forth in Section 3 below.
- 1.F.
- 1.F.1. Project Gutenberg volunteers and employees expend considerable
- effort to identify, do copyright research on, transcribe and proofread
- works not protected by U.S. copyright law in creating the Project
- Gutenberg-tm collection. Despite these efforts, Project Gutenberg-tm
- electronic works, and the medium on which they may be stored, may
- contain "Defects," such as, but not limited to, incomplete, inaccurate
- or corrupt data, transcription errors, a copyright or other
- intellectual property infringement, a defective or damaged disk or
- other medium, a computer virus, or computer codes that damage or
- cannot be read by your equipment.
- 1.F.2. LIMITED WARRANTY, DISCLAIMER OF DAMAGES - Except for the "Right
- of Replacement or Refund" described in paragraph 1.F.3, the Project
- Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation, the owner of the Project
- Gutenberg-tm trademark, and any other party distributing a Project
- Gutenberg-tm electronic work under this agreement, disclaim all
- liability to you for damages, costs and expenses, including legal
- fees. YOU AGREE THAT YOU HAVE NO REMEDIES FOR NEGLIGENCE, STRICT
- LIABILITY, BREACH OF WARRANTY OR BREACH OF CONTRACT EXCEPT THOSE
- PROVIDED IN PARAGRAPH 1.F.3. YOU AGREE THAT THE FOUNDATION, THE
- TRADEMARK OWNER, AND ANY DISTRIBUTOR UNDER THIS AGREEMENT WILL NOT BE
- LIABLE TO YOU FOR ACTUAL, DIRECT, INDIRECT, CONSEQUENTIAL, PUNITIVE OR
- INCIDENTAL DAMAGES EVEN IF YOU GIVE NOTICE OF THE POSSIBILITY OF SUCH
- DAMAGE.
- 1.F.3. LIMITED RIGHT OF REPLACEMENT OR REFUND - If you discover a
- defect in this electronic work within 90 days of receiving it, you can
- receive a refund of the money (if any) you paid for it by sending a
- written explanation to the person you received the work from. If you
- received the work on a physical medium, you must return the medium
- with your written explanation. The person or entity that provided you
- with the defective work may elect to provide a replacement copy in
- lieu of a refund. If you received the work electronically, the person
- or entity providing it to you may choose to give you a second
- opportunity to receive the work electronically in lieu of a refund. If
- the second copy is also defective, you may demand a refund in writing
- without further opportunities to fix the problem.
- 1.F.4. Except for the limited right of replacement or refund set forth
- in paragraph 1.F.3, this work is provided to you 'AS-IS', WITH NO
- OTHER WARRANTIES OF ANY KIND, EXPRESS OR IMPLIED, INCLUDING BUT NOT
- LIMITED TO WARRANTIES OF MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR ANY PURPOSE.
- 1.F.5. Some states do not allow disclaimers of certain implied
- warranties or the exclusion or limitation of certain types of
- damages. If any disclaimer or limitation set forth in this agreement
- violates the law of the state applicable to this agreement, the
- agreement shall be interpreted to make the maximum disclaimer or
- limitation permitted by the applicable state law. The invalidity or
- unenforceability of any provision of this agreement shall not void the
- remaining provisions.
- 1.F.6. INDEMNITY - You agree to indemnify and hold the Foundation, the
- trademark owner, any agent or employee of the Foundation, anyone
- providing copies of Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works in
- accordance with this agreement, and any volunteers associated with the
- production, promotion and distribution of Project Gutenberg-tm
- electronic works, harmless from all liability, costs and expenses,
- including legal fees, that arise directly or indirectly from any of
- the following which you do or cause to occur: (a) distribution of this
- or any Project Gutenberg-tm work, (b) alteration, modification, or
- additions or deletions to any Project Gutenberg-tm work, and (c) any
- Defect you cause.
- Section 2. Information about the Mission of Project Gutenberg-tm
- Project Gutenberg-tm is synonymous with the free distribution of
- electronic works in formats readable by the widest variety of
- computers including obsolete, old, middle-aged and new computers. It
- exists because of the efforts of hundreds of volunteers and donations
- from people in all walks of life.
- Volunteers and financial support to provide volunteers with the
- assistance they need are critical to reaching Project Gutenberg-tm's
- goals and ensuring that the Project Gutenberg-tm collection will
- remain freely available for generations to come. In 2001, the Project
- Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation was created to provide a secure
- and permanent future for Project Gutenberg-tm and future
- generations. To learn more about the Project Gutenberg Literary
- Archive Foundation and how your efforts and donations can help, see
- Sections 3 and 4 and the Foundation information page at
- www.gutenberg.org
- Section 3. Information about the Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation
- The Project Gutenberg Literary Archive Foundation is a non profit
- 501(c)(3) educational corporation organized under the laws of the
- state of Mississippi and granted tax exempt status by the Internal
- Revenue Service. The Foundation's EIN or federal tax identification
- number is 64-6221541. Contributions to the Project Gutenberg Literary
- Archive Foundation are tax deductible to the full extent permitted by
- U.S. federal laws and your state's laws.
- The Foundation's principal office is in Fairbanks, Alaska, with the
- mailing address: PO Box 750175, Fairbanks, AK 99775, but its
- volunteers and employees are scattered throughout numerous
- locations. Its business office is located at 809 North 1500 West, Salt
- Lake City, UT 84116, (801) 596-1887. Email contact links and up to
- date contact information can be found at the Foundation's web site and
- official page at www.gutenberg.org/contact
- For additional contact information:
- Dr. Gregory B. Newby
- Chief Executive and Director
- gbnewby@pglaf.org
- Section 4. Information about Donations to the Project Gutenberg
- Literary Archive Foundation
- Project Gutenberg-tm depends upon and cannot survive without wide
- spread public support and donations to carry out its mission of
- increasing the number of public domain and licensed works that can be
- freely distributed in machine readable form accessible by the widest
- array of equipment including outdated equipment. Many small donations
- ($1 to $5,000) are particularly important to maintaining tax exempt
- status with the IRS.
- The Foundation is committed to complying with the laws regulating
- charities and charitable donations in all 50 states of the United
- States. Compliance requirements are not uniform and it takes a
- considerable effort, much paperwork and many fees to meet and keep up
- with these requirements. We do not solicit donations in locations
- where we have not received written confirmation of compliance. To SEND
- DONATIONS or determine the status of compliance for any particular
- state visit www.gutenberg.org/donate
- While we cannot and do not solicit contributions from states where we
- have not met the solicitation requirements, we know of no prohibition
- against accepting unsolicited donations from donors in such states who
- approach us with offers to donate.
- International donations are gratefully accepted, but we cannot make
- any statements concerning tax treatment of donations received from
- outside the United States. U.S. laws alone swamp our small staff.
- Please check the Project Gutenberg Web pages for current donation
- methods and addresses. Donations are accepted in a number of other
- ways including checks, online payments and credit card donations. To
- donate, please visit: www.gutenberg.org/donate
- Section 5. General Information About Project Gutenberg-tm electronic works.
- Professor Michael S. Hart was the originator of the Project
- Gutenberg-tm concept of a library of electronic works that could be
- freely shared with anyone. For forty years, he produced and
- distributed Project Gutenberg-tm eBooks with only a loose network of
- volunteer support.
- Project Gutenberg-tm eBooks are often created from several printed
- editions, all of which are confirmed as not protected by copyright in
- the U.S. unless a copyright notice is included. Thus, we do not
- necessarily keep eBooks in compliance with any particular paper
- edition.
- Most people start at our Web site which has the main PG search
- facility: www.gutenberg.org
- This Web site includes information about Project Gutenberg-tm,
- including how to make donations to the Project Gutenberg Literary
- Archive Foundation, how to help produce our new eBooks, and how to
- subscribe to our email newsletter to hear about new eBooks.